Next Article in Journal
A Cardiovascular Risk Score for Use in Occupational Medicine
Next Article in Special Issue
Relationship between Behavioral Infant Speech Perception and Hearing Age for Children with Hearing Loss
Previous Article in Journal
Myeloid-Derived Suppressor Cells and Mesenchymal Stem/Stromal Cells in Myeloid Malignancies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Outcomes of Universal Newborn Screening Programs: Systematic Review

by
Christine Yoshinaga-Itano
1,*,†,
Vinaya Manchaiah
2,† and
Cynthia Hunnicutt
1,†
1
Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, UCB 594, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
2
Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX 77710, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10(13), 2784; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132784
Submission received: 15 May 2021 / Revised: 8 June 2021 / Accepted: 10 June 2021 / Published: 24 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Newborn Hearing Loss: Detection and Intervention)

Abstract

:
Background: This systematic review examined the outcomes (age of identification and intervention, developmental outcomes, cost-effectiveness, and adverse effects on parents) of universal newborn hearing screening (UNHS) for children with permanent congenital hearing loss (PCHL). Materials and methods: Multiple electronic databases were interrogated in March and April 2020 with further reports identified from article citations and unpublished literature. UNHS reports in English with comparisons of outcomes of infants who were not screened, and infants identified through other hearing screening programs. Results: 30 eligible reports from 14 populations with 7,325,138 infants screened through UNHS from 1616 non-duplicate references were included. UNHS results in a lower age of identification, amplification, and the initiation of early intervention services and better language/literacy development. Better speech perception/production were shown in younger, but not in older, children with early identification after UNHS. No significant findings were found for behavior problems and quality of life. UNHS was found to be cost-effective in terms of savings to society. In addition, no significant parental harm was noted as a result of UNHS. Conclusions: In highly developed countries, significantly better outcomes were found for children identified early through UNHS programs. Early language development predicts later literacy and language development.

1. Introduction

Studies from high-income countries (HICs) estimate that 1 in 1000 children are born with permanent childhood hearing loss (PCHL). However, children who are born in low/middle income countries (LMIC), as well as those children who were admitted to neonatal intensive care unit (NICU), have a much higher prevalence [1,2]. Despite the low prevalence, the PCHL could have severe negative consequences both at familial as well as societal levels.
Prior to establishing Universal Newborn Hearing Screening (UNHS) programs, the average language, literacy, social-emotional, and speech development of children with permanent childhood hearing loss (PCHL) was significantly lower than their peers with normal hearing. Eighteen-year-old children with hearing loss in the United States who were in the 12th grade between 1974–2003, had average literacy proficiency, between 3rd and 4th grade levels, more than two standard deviations below the developmental functioning of their hearing peers [3]. Wouters et al. [4] reported that deaf children in the Netherlands had the mean reading levels of first grade hearing students [2].
UNHS programs began to be implemented in the early 1990s, and by the end of the 1990s, there was evidence that these programs resulted in an earlier identification of hearing loss, earlier amplification, and earlier enrollment into early intervention services and that they significantly improved developmental outcomes in early childhood [5]. By 2007–8, evidence was deemed sufficient to established UNHS programs throughout most HICs and in some countries with fewer resources [6,7,8,9]. The quality benchmarks followed by most countries are those recommended by the Joint Committee on Infant Hearing [10,11]. However, due to a lack of endorsement from an international association such as the World Health Organization (WHO), countries make their own policy decisions.
The Guidelines Development Group (GDG) of the WHO is responsible for developing clinical guidelines based on evidence which is then endorsed by the WHO. Following this, the WHO member countries are encouraged to adopt the recommendations in their own country and/or context. The GDG has recently been considering endorsing UNHS provided that there are positive outcomes reported by the academic literature examined using a systematic review. Due to a lack of any recent reviews on wider outcomes for UNHS, this review was commissioned.
This systematic review was aimed at examining the outcomes of UNHS for children with PCHL. The key outcome indicators included: (a) early identification and intervention (i.e., age at dentification, age at amplification, age at intervention start), (b) developmental outcomes including receptive/expressive language, speech perception/production, literacy, social development, behavioral problems, and quality of life, (c) cost-effectiveness, and (d) adverse effects on parents of children with PCHL.

2. Materials and Methods

This systematic review was prospectively registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO 2020 CRD42020175451) [12]. The review methods were guided by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [13]. The authors of the study wrote the review protocol and performed all aspects of the review, although the WHO provided some input to the review design in order to ensure that the review met the key questions needed to be answered for the GDG to make a policy decision.

2.1. PCHL Definition

All children with a bilateral hearing loss of 20 dB or greater in their better ear were included in the studies reviewed within this systematic review.

2.2. Search Strategy

To identify eligible studies in March/April 2020, two reviewers (CYI, CH) interrogated electronic databases (PubMed, Medline (OVIDSP), Cochrane library, Google Scholar, Web of Science and One Search). Further reports were identified from citations of included papers and published literature (April 2020). Text-word searches along with MeSH terms or Subject Headings, were used to construct database searches (see Table 1). There were no date restrictions. All published reports were considered for inclusion if the abstract was in English. Searches of unpublished literature included relevant screening program reports.

2.3. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

We included reports of programs with children with PCHL identified as a result of established UNHS programs with comparisons to children with PCHL identified as a result of targeted/risk screening, children with PCHL not identified through hearing screening programs, and children with PCHL identified as a result of distraction screening. Studies that included developmental outcomes of language, auditory/speech production/perception, literacy, social-emotional, cognitive, or quality of life were included. The eligibility criteria were selected to address the research questions with reference to Participants, Intervention, Comparators, Outcomes, Study designs, and Timings (PICOST) [14,15] as shown in Table 2.

2.4. Article Selection and Data Extraction

Two independent evaluators extracted articles that met the inclusion criteria. First, articles were selected on the basis of titles and abstracts. Second, both independent evaluators read articles that appeared to have relevance to the systematic review questions and selected articles that met the criteria. Third, two independent evaluators compared selected articles and discussed articles that were not on both lists. A third reviewer (VM) evaluated all articles with a discrepancy between reviewers 1 and 2 for inclusion. After discussion, a final list of final articles was chosen. Figure 1 presents the PRISMA flow diagram of different phases through the systematic review.

2.5. Synthesis of Included Studies

Most articles report very different developmental outcomes using a range of outcome measures. For this reason, a quantitative synthesis was not possible. A descriptive synthesis of the included articles was performed as outlined and described by Campbell et al. [15] and Popay et al. [16] to answer the specific questions.

2.6. Quality Analysis and Level of Evidence

The Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP) checklist was used to evaluate the quality of included studies [17]. CASP provides a specific checklist for different study designs (https://casp-uk.net/casp-tools-checklists/; accessed on 1 May 2021) and a relevant version of the checklist (cohort, case-control, and economic evaluation). The checklist contained questions on several sections that enable a structured approach to finding evidence, determining a possible sources of bias, and evaluating the internal and external validity of each study.
Assessment of the level of evidence for each outcome was rated according to the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) protocol [18]. The GRADE level of evidence includes four levels: (a) high quality, (b) moderate quality, (c) low quality, or (d) very low quality. Higher scores are indicative of more confidence in the cumulative evidence. The appraisal of studies was performed independently by two researchers (CYI and VM) and any discrepancies were resolved by discussion with the third researcher (CH).

3. Results

3.1. Study Characteristics

Table 3 provides a summary of included studies as related to four key outcomes. Of the 30 included studies, 7 studies were focused on early identification and intervention, 11 studies were focused on developmental outcomes, 4 studies included cost-effectiveness, and 9 studies were focused on the adverse effects on parents of children with PCHL.

3.1.1. Question 1: Early identification and Intervention

Does UNHS Lower the Age of Identification and Increase the Number of Children Identified Early?

UNHS lowers the age of identification when compared to risk factor screen (RFS) and no screen (NS) groups. The research across the globe did not report their statistics consistently. Some research reported the proportion/percentage of the cohorts below and above a specific benchmark, whereas others have reported a mean/median for the different cohorts [19,22,23,25,38]. See Supplementary Tables S1 and S2.
Cohort Comparisons: A prospective cohort study from the United Kingdom (UK) indicated that for children identified as a result of UNHS, the proportion of children for whom the age of identification was less than 9 months was 5 times greater than for those without UNHS [38]. The no-UNHS group had children identified through a distraction screen implemented in the UK after the children were 7 months of age. In a United States (US) study, Yoshinaga-Itano et al. [25] reported the results of the population of the state of Colorado n = 274. A matched sample of screen (n = 25) and NS (n = 25) groups had a median age of identification of 5 weeks for UNHS and 24 months for the NS group; 84% of the UNHS group were identified by 6 months while only 8% of the NS group were identified by 6 months of age. More recently, in an Australian quasi-randomized cohort study [22], a comparison was made between UNHS, RFS and NS screen and the results showed that the mean age of identification for UNHS was 9 months, (comparable to Kennedy et al. [38]) for RFS it was 18 months, and for NS it was 24 months. In a US cohort study, Sininger et al. [23] in California investigated a group of 64 children with hearing loss, 47 in the UNHS group and 17 in the NS group. The median age of identification for the UNHS group was 3 months (2.4 months for the Fail group and 22.5 months for the Pass group) and 27.8 months for the NS group.
Single Program Descriptions: UNHS population statistics from the established program in the UK report that the median age of identification is significantly lower than the 1993–1996 UK birth cohort [38], as well as the no-screen cohort. Uus and Bamford [20] reported that the median age of identification for their UK cohort was 10 weeks. In another study, Weichbold and Wehlz-Mueller [39], a retrospective chart review of an Ear Nose and Throat department in Austria, reported a mean of 9.7 months for UNHS and 46 months for NS. The median for UNHS was 4 months as compared to a median 37.4 months for the NS group.
Population Description: Wood et al. [21] reported population statistics for the UK UNHS program from 2006–2013. The median age of identification was 49 days.
Overall, the scientific evidence suggests that the UNHS lowers the age at which hearing loss is identified (see Supplementary Table S3). Regardless of the way that the data have been reported, all studies have found significantly lower ages of identification of congenital hearing loss. Most comparison studies found the age of identification with UNHS within the first few months of life possible, particularly with maturity of the UNHS system, while NS populations were identified at a mean/median age of 24 months or older. The systems across the world are quite different and the ages of identification vary dramatically by the date of publication and the newness of the UNHS program.

Does UNHS Lower the Age of Amplification Fit?

Comparisons between three Australian cohorts were made by Wake et al. [22], who found a lower age of amplification fit for UNHS 13.5 months when compared to 17.9 months in RFS group and also 24 months in NS group. Korver et al. [31] found that children in the Netherlands identified as a result of UNHS were fit, with an amplification at 15.7 months as compared to 29.2 months for children in the NS group. In the UK Wessex study, Kennedy et al. [19] reported a median age at amplification fit of 15 months for a 1993–1996 birth cohort. The UK has made significant progress towards lowering the age at amplification since 1993. Uus and Bamford [20] reported that a median age at amplification fit was 16 weeks. In a population study report for the UK for birth cohorts from 2006–2013, Wood et al. [21] reported a median age of amplification fit at 82 days. In a New York state study in the US, Dalzell, et al. [24] reported a median age of amplification fit at 7.5 months. Age of amplification for both UNHS and NS groups was within 3 months after the identification of hearing loss. Sininger et al. [23] reported a median age of amplification fit of 5.6 months for all UNHS and 4.9 months for the UNHS fails compared to a median of 29.1 months for the NS group.
Overall, the studies that were reviewed included comparisons of cohorts of UNHS, targeted screened, distraction screened, and NS or opportunistic identification (see Supplementary Table S3). All studies regardless of the risk of bias or quality found that UNHS lowers the age at which children are fit with amplification.

Does UNHS Lower the Age at Which Early Intervention Services Are Initiated?

Kennedy et al. [38], from a prospective cohort study in the UK, found that the odds of initiating education management had an 8 times greater chance of being initiated before 9 months with UNHS than without. Sininger et al. [23] reported a mean age of 10.58 months for early intervention service initiation, with a median of 8.9 months for UNHS and median 30.5 months for NS. In the US, Yoshinaga-Itano et al. [25] reported that 87% of all participants initiated early intervention within two months of the age of identification, which would be a mean of 3.1 months for UNHS as compared to a mean of 24 months for the NS groups. Dalzell et al. [24] reported a mean age of early intervention initiation at 3 months. Uus and Bamford [20] reported a mean of early intervention service initiation at 10 weeks (2.5 months) Wood et al. [21] reported a median age of referral to early intervention services of 50 days.
All studies showed that UNHS lowers the age at which early intervention services are initiated, whether they were cohort comparison studies, single cohorts after implementation of UNHS and population studies. In summary, as UNHS programs mature, the age of identification, age at amplification and age at initiation of early intervention services have dropped significantly. Refer to Supplementary Table S3 for further details.

3.1.2. Question 2: Developmental Outcomes

Does UNHS Improve Receptive and Expressive Language?

Kennedy et al. [38] reported higher adjusted mean z scores for language as compared with non-verbal ability for both confirmation by nine months of age and birth during UNHS, although z scores for expressive language were not significantly higher for children 5.4–11.7 years of age. Worsfold et al. [27] found that earlier confirmed hearing loss was associated with significantly more sentences and categories of high-pitched and morphological markers at a mean age of 7 years 7 months. The odds ratio for higher performance of early identified children was 3.03. No differences between early-identified and late-identified children were found for the number of categories of low-pitched morphological markers, poorer logical simplifications and sentences with multiple clauses.
Wake et al. [22] compared the outcomes of UNHS with risk factor screen (RFS) at the test age 5–6 years of age for both and no screen (NS) (test age 7–8 years) for receptive, expressive language and receptive vocabulary. For children without intellectual disability, significantly higher language quotients for UNHS were found with 88.9 (UNHS), 83 (RFS), 81.8 (NS) for receptive language, 89.3 (UNHS), 80.7 (RFS), and 74.9 (NS) for expressive language, and 91.5 (UNHS), 83.8 (RFS) and 79.4 (NS) for receptive vocabulary. Significant differences were found between language levels and cognitive levels.
Korver et al. [31] reported that when comparing outcomes of children at the test age 3–5 years participating in UNHS and those participating in distraction screen, children identified through UNHS did not show significantly better receptive and expressive language quotients or significantly more words produced. The age of identification of children through UNHS was far from the Early Hearing Detection and Intervention (EHDI) 1-3-6 (screen by 1 month, identify by 3 months and in early intervention by 6 months) guidelines.
Sininger et al. [32] found that age-of-fit predicted better receptive language at a test age of 3–5 years. Each month lag accounted for a 0.17 months delay in receptive language and a 0.30 months lag in expressive language.
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. [33] found that children between the age of 9 months to 5 years who were screened had a receptive language quotient mean of 82.9 and an expressive language quotient mean of 81.5 as compared to the NS group with a receptive language quotient mean of 62.1 and an expressive language quotient mean of 66.8. Children born before 1992 and UNHS had a total language quotient of 55.7. In addition, children who were born during a period of UNHS had a 95.5 word produced mean as compared to 14.5 word produced mean for the no screen group. Children born during periods of screen had a mean of 30 different words versus a mean of 7 different words for the NS group. Children identified as deaf or hard of hearing born in hospitals that screened versus hospitals that did not screen for hearing were 2.54 times more likely to have language quotients within the normal range 80 or greater; 82.4% of the children born in hospitals that screened had language quotients within the normal range compared to 68.4% of the children born in hospitals that did not screen for hearing.
Overall, children identified through UNHS had better receptive/expressive language and receptive/expressive vocabulary as long as the children were early-identified before 9 months of age. Refer to Supplementary Tables S4–S6 for further details

Does UNHS Improve Speech Perception and Speech Production?

Sininger et al. [32] found that age of fit predicted better speech discrimination from 3 to 5 years of age. Sininger et al. [32] also found that age of fit predicted better speech intelligibility. Both degree of hearing loss and age of fit predicted better word and sentence articulation from 3 to 5 years of age. Very few studies have demonstrated that UNHS improves speech perception and speech production and those that have are studies of children in the first five years of life. Overall, the literature indicates that the timeline for speech production and speech perception may be longer than the sensitive period for the development of language skills.
Speech scores did not differ significantly for either exposure to UNHS or early confirmation of hearing loss in the Kennedy et al. study for children tested at 5.4–11.7 years [19].
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. [33] reported that children aged 9 to 61 months of age who had been screened had significantly more consonants, consonant blends and better speech intelligibility than children who were not screened. See Supplementary Table S6 for further details.

Does UNHS Improve Literacy?

The prospective Wessex study reported literacy results of children between 13 and 19 years of age [32]. The primary predictors of literacy for this group of children were age of identification, maternal level of education, cognitive level and degree of hearing loss. EID children were slightly more than one half a standard deviation (SD) (0.63 SD) below the mean of the normally hearing control. The LID children were almost 2 SDs below the mean of the normal hearing control children (1.74 SD). Participation in UNHS was no longer a predictor after age of identification was introduced as a predictor. In this cohort, UNHS was effective only if children were early-identified. The EID group maintained the same half SD gap found at the earlier age of 5.4–11.7 years [30]. EID in this study was before 9 months versus after 9 months of age.
At the age of 6 to 10 years, McCann et al. [26] reported that UNHS participation predicted a higher aggregate reading (0.39 adjusted mean difference) and adaptive behavior communication scores, (0.51 adjusted mean difference) vs no UNHS. The EID by 9 months had significantly higher adjusted mean scores for aggregate reading (0.51), for basic reading (0.55), for reading comprehension (0.48) and adjusted mean on the communication scale (0.56) than the later-detection group. Benefits to reading and communication were partially mediated by better language ability (0.51) and communication (0.56) than the later-detection group. Benefits to reading and communication were partially mediated by better language ability. Overall, children identified through UNHS had significantly better reading comprehension from early childhood through late adolescence, as long as the children are EID at least before 9 months of age. Refer to Tables S4–S6 for further details.

Does UNHS Improve Social Development?

Korver et al. [31] found that children, assessed between 3 to 5 years of age, identified through UNHS, had social development quotients 8.8 quotient points higher than those identified through distraction testing.
Stevenson et al. [28] found no significant differences in children with a mean age of 7 years and 11 months by age of confirmation before and after 9 months on daily living skills and socialization, though children with hearing loss had significantly lower functioning than children with typical hearing. Lower socialization scores for both children with hearing loss and those with typical hearing were related to language development. Stevenson et al. [29] examined the relationship between children 6 to 10 years at Time 1 and 13–20 years at Time 2 in children with hearing loss using spoken language and found significant relationships between language at Time 1 and emotional behavioral problems at Time 2. See Supplementary Table S6 for further details.

Does UNHS Reduce Behavior Problems?

Stevenson et al. [28] for the Wessex study found no significant differences in behavior problems in children between 5 and 11 years of age, by age of identification less than 9 months of age versus greater than 9 months or participation in UNHS versus NS groups. Lower behavior problems were related to higher language levels. Level of behavior problems of the children with hearing loss was significantly higher than the children with normal hearing. Higher language levels were related to fewer behavior problems. Wake et al. [22] found no differences in behavior problems between children in the UNHS, RFS, and NS groups. Overall, these studies provide mixed evidence for a connection between UNHS and behavioral problems. See Supplementary Table S6 for further details.

Does UNHS Improve Quality of Life?

Wake et al. [22] found no differences in quality of life between children in the UNHS, RFS and NS groups. In another study, Korver et al. [31] found that pediatric quality of life was 5.3 times higher for children identified through UNHS than distraction screen. Age of identification information was not provided. Age at amplification for UNHS was 15.7 months as compared to 29.2 months for distraction screen. No studies were found that compared the quality of life of children who were screened and met EHDI 1-3-6 guidelines. Those studies including children identified through UNHS had very late ages of identification and differed in the results. Wake et al. [22] showed no significant differences, while Korver et al. [31] showed better quality of life for children identified through UNHS than distraction screening. However, it is likely that the quality of life is associated with language, literacy, and vocational outcomes. See Supplementary Table S6 for further details.

3.1.3. Question 3: Cost Effectiveness

Is UNHS Cost Effective and Is There a Cost Benefit?

Mehl and Thomson [36] compared the cost of UNHS from 1992–1996. Costs of the Colorado UNHS included prevalence of hearing loss, false-positive rate, positive predictive value, and sensitivity of the screening. These UNHS costs were compared to the costs of other newborn screening for congenital diseases. The costs of early intervention and other educational costs were included. The cost of identifying each case of congenital hearing loss was approximately $9600 per child. If only half of the children who are deaf or hard of hearing realized some ultimate savings in school-based costs because of UNHS and early amplification, the UNHS program would recover all screening costs after only 10 years through subsequent savings in avoided intervention. Recovery of all initial costs (and subsequent cost savings) was independent of improved developmental outcomes. Mehl and Thomson [36] projected a 50 percent reduction in education costs.
Keren et al. [37] worked from the assumption that education costs would be reduced by 10 percent if 50–70% had language in the typical range due to UNHS compared to 28–40% if children were later-identified and estimated a lifetime cost savings of $430,000 per individual.
Schroeder et al. [34] conducted a study in the UK of the actual economic costs of congenital bilateral PCHL for children between 7 and 9 years of age. Unit costs were applied to estimates of health, social, and broader resource use made by 120 children with PCHL and 65 children in a normally hearing comparison. The mean annual societal cost was £14,092 British pounds for children with PCHL compared with £4206 British pounds for children with normal hearing, a cost difference of £9885 British pounds. After adjusting for severity and other potential confounders in a linear regression model, mean societal costs among children with PCHL were reduced by £2553 British pounds for each unit increase in the z score for receptive language. Exposure to UNHS was associated with a smaller cost reduction of £2213 British pounds. The best estimate of annual cost saving of UNHS in middle childhood is 21% of the neonatal cost of UNHS per child with PCHL in the United Kingdom. If such an annual cost saving were generalizable across other years of the child’s school life, this would support an economic argument in favor of UNHS.
Chorozoglou et al. [35] in a UK study of adolescents, aged 13–20 years (n = 110; 73 with PCHL and 37 with normal hearing), performed a follow-through of the Wessex study of 157,000 births in Southern England in which half were exposed to UNHS. The study found that the mean annual costs for PCHL were £15,914 British pounds as compared to the £5883 British pounds cost of children with normal hearing annually. The difference was an annual cost of £10,031 British pounds. Costs for the education of children with PCHL decreased by £1616 British pounds each year with an increase of one unit in receptive language z-score.
Overall, studies in the US and the UK have demonstrated the cost-effectiveness of UNHS in terms of savings to society (see Supplementary Table S7).

3.1.4. Question 4: Adverse Effects

Does UNHS Cause Social and/or Emotional Harm (e.g., Worry, Stress, Anxiety) to Parents (Mothers)?

Tueller and White [40] and Tueller [48] conducted a study of 192 mothers whose babies were screened in 11 hospitals in Utah. Of these 192 mothers, at Time 1 (immediately after UNHS), 83 had an initial pass, 34 had a Fail/Pass 2nd screen, 9 had Fail/Fail 2nd screen and 66 did not know the results of the screen. At Time 2 (within 6 weeks after the initial screen), 95 of 192 responded; 60/83 had an initial pass, 18/34 had Fail/Pass, 7/9 had Fail/Fail and 10/66 had an unknown result. Parental infant health concerns were assessed through a survey. Maternal anxiety and vulnerability were measured. Mothers worried as much about other aspects of the infant’s health as hearing concerns. Mothers of children who initially failed UNHS were slightly more worried, but the worry disappeared by Time 2. There was no significant difference in maternal anxiety between mothers whose infants passed and mothers whose infants failed UNHS.
Watkins et al. [41] reported that 288 of 290 mothers enrolled in the study. Of these, 49 failed in both ears and 79 failed in one ear. In Stage 1, 288 surveyed within days of UNHS for screen attitudes, satisfaction and anxiety. In Stage 2, 57 were assessed at 6 weeks, and in Stage 3, 61 were assessed at 9 months. There was no significant difference between responses of the 288 surveyed immediately after UNHS and those assessed at 9-months. About 1% of participants expressed worry. Crockett et al. [42] sent invitations to 722 mothers. They had a 53% (n = 344) response rate. In Group 1, 103 passed at either Screen 1 or Screen 2. In Group 2, 81 passed at Screen 3. In Group 3, 105 referred on one ear at Screen 3 to audiological assessment. In Group 4, 55 referred at Screen 3 in both ears to audiological assessment. A measure of anxiety was sent 3 weeks after the screen. The return rate was 65% for Group 1 and 57% for Group 2. Mean anxiety levels were in the normal range. Anxiety and worry increased significantly as the number of tests increased. There was a significant interaction between the amount of worry and the mother’s belief that no clear response did not mean that the child had a hearing loss. Anxiety of mothers in Group 4 was related to this belief.
Crockett et al. [43] compared UNHS and Health Visitor Distraction Testing (HVDT) responses and found that there were no significant differences in mother’s anxiety and worry. There was higher satisfaction from UNHS than from HVDT and higher positive attitudes after satisfactory screen result from UNHS than HVDT. Vohr et al. [44] invited 307 parents, at the first screen 80% and at the second screen 90% agreed to participate. 88–89% reported no maternal worry, whereas the remaining 9–10% reported being worried. Mothers at greater risk of being worried were socially disadvantaged and less aware of UNHS. Kolski et al. [45] compared maternal anxiety from a cohort of 3202 children screened at birth and 2588 screened at 2 months of age in France. Screening at birth coverage was significantly higher for the infant screened at birth (95.7%) compared to those screened at two months of age (64.2%). The false positive rate was 0.29% for the first strategy compared to 2.65% for screening at 2 months of age. One hundred and forty three mother-infant pairs participated in psychological assessment for maternal anxiety and quality of interaction. No significant differences were found for the NS group as compared to either of the two screening strategies. Khairi et al. [46] reported a study of 78 mothers whose infants had a positive finding for the first screen and 50 mothers prior to a second screen were assessed for maternal anxiety. Ten percent of the mothers were found to have moderate anxiety and 8% were found to have severe anxiety after the first screen which dropped to 4% before the 2nd screen. These findings were consistent with the study by Stuart et al. [47].
Overall, these studies investigating whether or not UNHS caused parental harm have found that very little stress, anxiety and worry have been identified whether utilizing customized non-standardized questionnaires or standardized instruments of maternal anxiety and stress (see Supplementary Table S8). The levels of parental stress and anxiety are comparable to those reported by mothers of healthy newborns without hearing loss and amount of concern about the hearing screening is of significantly less concern than concerns about other health matters.

3.2. Quality Analysis and Level of Evidence

3.2.1. Quality Analysis of Included Studies

The quality assessment of individual studies was assessed using the CASP checklist (see Table 4). All cohort studies addressed a clearly focused issue; however, the quality of studies regarding other criteria in the checklist varied. In particular, in most studies it was unclear if they identified and accounted for confounding factors (e.g., socioeconomic factors, parental education, support at home, learning environment). In addition, outcome measurement bias was also noted in nearly half of the studies. Issues were also noted in terms of not following the subjects completely and for a long-enough duration. Moreover, nearly half of the studies’ results were not precise enough. The only case-control study found to have a minimum bias with the only limitation being treating the experimental group and control group equally. Overall, despite some issues identified, several high-quality studies exist with minimum bias. The economic evaluation studies were found to have several issues and a higher risk of bias. Four of the six studies did not fully account for all important and relevant resources required for cost-benefit analysis. Most of the studies fully adjusted the costs for different times, although such an adjustment was not performed adequately in three of the included studies.

3.2.2. Determination of Level of Cumulative Evidence

The quality of the evidence underpinning the recommendations was graded using the GRADE (see Table 5). The GRADE rating for “developmental outcomes,” “cost benefit,” and also “negative side effects” was very low. However, the GRADE rating for “early identification and intervention” was low. Almost all studies included in the review were observational in nature. Randomization and blinding were not possible given the nature of the intervention. The ratings were not downgraded for lack of blinding because the outcome was deemed to be less potentially influenced. In general, the studies for “developmental outcomes,” and “negative side effects” showed a limited bias and also showed moderate association with consistent results across studies, although some inconsistencies were noted for studies in the outcomes “early identification and intervention” and “societal cost-benefit”.

4. Discussion

The implementation of UNHS was started nearly three decades ago. However, only a few HICs provide UNHS universally. In most countries, especially LMIC, the newborn hearing screening (NHS) is not universal and is only available to some populations, primarily through private hospitals. This may be attributed to the lack of a strong policy to implement the UNHS as well as issues related to cost to healthcare. This comprehensive review provides an update on evidence related to outcomes of UNHS.
Our review identified positive outcomes on all the four outcomes this review targeted. UNHS results in lower age of identification [22,25,38] and lower age of amplification, as well as lower age of initiation of early intervention services [22,25,38] when compared to targeted/risk screen, distraction screen and no screen/opportunistic identification. Developmental outcomes including speech perception/production, receptive/expressive language, literacy, social development, and quality of life were better in children identified early through UNHS are higher than those identified later [21,26,30,33,38]. In particular, children who are EID have significantly better literacy outcomes at both 5–11 years and 13–17 years of age than those that are LID [26,30]. Although the cost-effectiveness studies were limited to those from the UK and the US, they demonstrated the cost-effectiveness of UNHS in terms of savings to society [34,35,36,37]. Finally, the studies focusing on adverse effects show that the UNHS does not result in any significant harm to the parents of children who undergo UNHS [34,35,40,41,48]. Taken these studies together, there is long-standing evidence to demonstrate the positive outcomes of UNHS.
It is noteworthy that the outcome studies that were included were predominantly from very HIC countries. This is because the studies from LMIC did not meet the inclusion criteria primarily because they were descriptions of a single cohort. However, descriptive studies from lower resourced countries replicated many of the outcomes reported [49,50,51,52,53]. These studies have demonstrated that more recent UNHS programs that have been established around the world, especially in LMIC, are making progress towards lowering the age of identification, age of amplification, and age at intervention and meeting 1-3-6 goals, despite the challenging socio-economic situations.

4.1. Comparison of UNHS Outcomes of Other Newborn Screening Programs

Most newborn screening programs, e.g., genetic/metabolic, involve a blood sample with multiple tests run on the same sample. In these programs, intervention after identification is within the medical system and begins through the primary medical provider. Visual screening and intervention are completed within the medical system. UNHS begins with screening in the neonatal period followed by audiologic follow-through for diagnosis and fitting of amplification technology. However, the primary intervention is within the educational system, initially involves parent education and is followed by direct child intervention within the educational system through 18 years of age.

4.2. Limitations of Existing Evidence and the Review Process

Despite the large amount of literature on UNHS from all across the globe, there are several limitations to the existing literature that limit applicability. First, there is a limited number of controlled studies and the quality of the included studies varied widely as identified by the quality analysis of CASP ratings. There are no randomized controlled trials. This is because it is not ethical to deny the opportunity of early identification of hearing impairment for newborns which will result in life-long consequences. Moreover, the quality analysis identified that many studies did not consider the potential confounders and the long-term outcomes were reported by only a few studies. Additionally, pediatric hearing loss is a low-incidence disability and even with thousands of infants screened, only about two per thousand are identified with hearing loss. Creative ways are needed to perform more controlled studies (e.g., cluster randomization) to strengthen the evidence base in this area. Second, the choice of outcome measure as well as the reporting of outcomes varied widely across the studies. Expert consensus is needed to agree on key outcomes that should be measured and reported when performing UNHS. In addition, researchers should use a standard reporting format (e.g., EQUATOR network guidelines [54]) to ensure that as many details about the study findings are provided when publishing scientific findings in this area. Finally, most of the studies from LMIC countries did not have a control group and hence did not meet the eligibility criteria for this review. More work is needed to develop high quality studies from LMIC as the context could play an important role in the outcomes of UNHS.
The current review was much broader in focus compared to other reviews pertaining to UNHS [1,2]. This was necessary to answer the questions raised by the GDG committee of the WHO when making policy recommendations. However, this broad nature of review did not allow us to do an in-depth analysis of the included studies. Moreover, the heterogeneity of studies in terms of study design as well as outcomes reporting did not allow us to perform a quantitative synthesis of included studies.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm10132784/s1: Table S1, Age of identification by type of screen; Table S2, Proportion by age of identification at 12, 9, 6, and 3 months; Table S3, Early identification, age of amplification and age at early intervention outcomes of UNHS; Table S4, Language/reading outcomes by UNHS vs no UNHS; Table S5, Language/reading outcomes by age of identification; Table S6, Developmental outcomes of UNHS; Table S7, Societal cost-benefit of UNHS, Table S8, Negative side effects (harm) of UNHS. References [55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80] are cited in the supplementary materials.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.Y.-I.; methodology, C.Y.-I., V.M. and C.H.; data curation, C.Y-I., V.M. and C.H.; writing original draft, C.Y.-I.; review and editing, C.Y.-I. and V.M.; funding acquisition, C.Y.-I. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the World Health Organization, WHO Registration number: 2020/1005769, PO: 202528946. The WHO Unit responsible for this contract: HQ/UHL UHC/Life Course for $5000 to the University of Colorado, Boulder to support the update of “WHO guidelines on postnatal care of the mother and the newborn. This publication was supported by the Disability Research and Dissemination Center (DRDC) through its Cooperative Agreement Number 5U01DD001007 from the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) and Prevention. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the DRDC or the CDC.

Institutional Reviewer Board Statement

Not applicable, because this study did not involve human subjects.

Acknowledgments

This systematic review was commissioned by the World Health Organization (WHO). The authors would like to acknowledge input from Karen Edmond, Shuchita Gupta, and Shelly Chadha for the development of the review protocol.

Conflicts of Interest

No authors have either a financial or non-financial conflict of interest related to the content of this submission.

Abbreviations

CASPCritical Appraisal Skills Programme
EIDEarly-identified
GRADEGrading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation
GDPGuideline Development Group
HVDTHealth Visitor Distraction Testing
LIDLate-identified
LMICLow Middle Income Countries
NICUNewborn intensive care unit
NSNo Screen
PCHLPermanent childhood hearing loss
PICOTSParticipants: Intervention, Comparators, Outcomes, Timings and Study
PRISMAPreferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-analyses
RFSRisk Factor Screen
SDStandard deviation
UNHSUniversal newborn hearing screening
UKUnited Kingdom
USUnited States
VHDVery highly developed
WHOWorld Hearing Organization
CYI(Christine Yoshinaga-Itano)
CH(Cynthia Hunnicutt)
VM(Vinaya Manchaiah)

References

  1. Butcher, E.; Dezateux, C.; Cortina-Borja, M.; Knowles, R.L. Prevalence of permanent childhood hearing loss detected at the universal newborn hearing screen: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0219600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bussé, A.M.L.; Hoeve, H.L.J.; Nasserinejad, K.; Mackey, A.R.; Simonsz, H.J.; Goedegebure, A. Prevalence of permanent neonatal hearing impairment: Systematic review and Bayesian meta-analysis. Int. J. Audiol. 2020, 59, 475–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Qi, S.; Mitchell, R.E. Large-scale academic achievement testing of deaf and hard-of-hearing students: Past, present, and future. J. Deaf. Stud. Deaf. Educ. 2011, 17, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Wauters, L.N.; Van Bon, W.H.J.; Tellings, A.E.J.M. Reading comprehension of Dutch deaf children. Read. Writ. 2006, 19, 49–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Nelson, H.D.; Bougatsos, C.; Nygren, P. Universal newborn hearing screening: Systematic review to update the 2001 US preventive services task force recommendation. Pediatrics 2008, 122, e266–e276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Davis, A.; Bamford, J.; Wilson, I.; Ramkalawan, T.; Forshaw, M.; Wright, S. A critical review of the role of neonatal hearing screening in the detection of congenital hearing impairment. Health Technol. Assess. 1997, 10, 1–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. The use of automated auditory brainstem response and otoacoustic emissions tests for newborn hearing screening. In IHE Report: Screening Newborns for Hearing; IHE: Edmonton, AB, Canada, 2007; p. 102.
  8. Merlin, T.; Hedayati, H.; Sullivan, T.; Buckley, E.; Newton, S.; Hodgkinson, B.; Bywood, P.; Jenner, F.; Moss, J. Universal Neonatal Hearing Screening; MSAC Reference 17 Assessment Report; MSAC: Canberra, Australia, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  9. Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. Year 2007 position statement: Principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. Pediatrics 2007, 120, 898–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. Year 2019 Position Statement: Principles and Guidelines for Early Hearing Detection and Intervention Programs. J. Early Hear. Detect. Interv. 2019, 4, 1–44. [Google Scholar]
  11. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Hunnicutt, C.; Manchaiah, V. A Systematic Review of the Evidence for the Effectiveness of Universal Newborn Hearing Screening. PROSPERO 2020, CRD42020175451. [Google Scholar]
  12. Schardt, C.; Adams, M.B.; Owens, T.; Keitz, S.; Fontelo, P. Utilization of the PICO Framework to Improve Searching PubMed for Clinical Questions. BMC Med. Inform. Decis. Mak. 2007, 7, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. BMJ 2009, 339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Akers, J.; University of New York, Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. Systematic Reviews: CRD’s Guidance for Undertaking Reviews in Health Care; CRD University of York: York, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  15. Campbell, M.; McKenzie, J.E.; Sowden, A.; Katikireddi, S.V.; Brennan, S.E.; Ellis, S.; Hartmann-Boyce, J.; Ryan, R.; Shepperd, S.; Thomas, J.; et al. Synthesis without meta-analysis (SWiM) in systematic reviews: Reporting guideline. BMJ 2020, 368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Popay, J.; Roberts, H.; Sowden, A.; Petticrew, M.; Arai, L.; Rodgers, M.; Britten, N.; Roen, K.; Duffy, S.; Guidance on the Conduct of Narrative Synthesis in Systematic Reviews. A Product from the ESRC Methods Programme. Version 1. Available online: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.178.3100&rep=rep1&type=pdf (accessed on 15 May 2021).
  17. Taylor, R.S.; Reeves, B.C.; Ewings, P.E.; Taylor, R.J. Critical appraisal skills training for health care professionals: A randomized controlled trial [ISRCTN46272378]. BMC Med. Educ. 2004, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Balshem, H.; Helfand, M.; Schünemann, H.J.; Oxman, A.D.; Kunz, R.; Brozek, J.; Vist, G.E.; Falck-Ytter, Y.; Meerpohl, J.; Norris, S. GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2011, 64, 401–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kennedy, C.R.; McCann, D.C.; Campbell, M.J.; Law, C.M.; Mullee, M.; Petrou, S.; Watkin, P.; Worsfold, S.; Yuen, H.M.; Stevenson, J. Language ability after early detection of permanent childhood hearing impairment. N. Engl. J. Med. 2006, 354, 2131–2141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Uus, K.; Bamford, J. Effectiveness of population-based newborn hearing screening in England: Ages of interventions and profile of cases. Pediatrics 2006, 117, e887–e893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Wood, S.A.; Sutton, G.J.; Davis, A.C. Performance and characteristics of the newborn hearing screening programme in England: The first seven years. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54, 353–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Wake, M.; Ching, T.Y.C.; Wirth, K.; Poulakis, Z.; Mensah, F.K.; Gold, L.; King, A.; Bryson, H.E.; Reilly, S.; Rickards, F. Population outcomes of three approaches to detection of congenital hearing loss. Pediatrics 2016, 137, e20151722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Sininger, Y.S.; Martinez, A.; Eisenberg, L.; Christensen, E.; Grimes, A.; Hu, J. Newborn hearing screening speeds diagnosis and access to intervention by 20–25 months. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 2009, 20, 049–057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Dalzell, L.; Orlando, M.; MacDonald, M.; Berg, A.; Bradley, M.; Cacace, A.; Campbell, D.; DeCristofaro, J.; Gravel, J.; Greenberg, E.; et al. The New York State universal newborn hearing screening demonstration project: Ages of hearing loss identification, hearing aid fitting, and enrollment in early intervention. Ear Hear. 2000, 21, 118–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Coulter, D.; Thomson, V. Developmental outcomes of children with hearing loss born in Colorado hospitals with and without universal newborn hearing screening programs. Semin. Neonatol. 2001, 6, 521–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. McCann, D.C.; Worsfold, S.; Law, C.M.; Mullee, M.; Petrou, S.; Stevenson, J.; Yuen, H.M.; Kennedy, C.R. Reading and communication skills after universal newborn screening for permanent childhood hearing impairment. Arch. Dis. Child. 2008, 94, 293–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Worsfold, S.; Mahon, M.; Yuen, H.M.; Kennedy, C. Narrative skills following early confirmation of permanent childhood hearing impairment. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2010, 52, 922–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Stevenson, J.; McCann, D.C.; Law, C.M.; Mullee, M.; Petrou, S.; Worsfold, S.; Yuen, H.M.; Kennedy, C.R. The effect of early confirmation of hearing loss on the behaviour in middle childhood of children with bilateral hearing impairment. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2010, 53, 269–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Stevenson, J.; Pimperton, H.; Kreppner, J.; Worsfold, S.; Terlektsi, E.; Mahon, M.; Kennedy, C. Language and reading comprehension in middle childhood predicts emotional and behaviour difficulties in adolescence for those with permanent childhood hearing loss. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 180–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Pimperton, H.; Blythe, H.; Kreppner, J.; Mahon, M.; Peacock, J.L.; Stevenson, J.; Terlektsi, E.; Worsfold, S.; Yuen, H.M.; Kennedy, C.R. The impact of universal newborn hearing screening on long-term literacy outcomes: A prospective cohort study. Arch. Dis. Child. 2016, 101, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Korver, A.M.H.; Konings, S.; Dekker, F.W.; Beers, M.; Wever, C.C.; Frijns, J.H.M.; Oudesluys-Murphy, A.M.; DECIBEL Collaborative Study Group. Newborn hearing screening vs later hearing screening and developmental outcomes in children with permanent childhood hearing impairment. JAMA 2010, 304, 1701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Sininger, Y.S.; Grimes, A.; Christensen, E. Auditory development in early amplified children: Factors influencing auditory-based communication outcomes in children with hearing loss. Ear Hear. 2010, 31, 166–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Coulter, D.; Thomson, V. The Colorado Newborn Hearing Screening Project: Effects on speech and language development for children with hearing loss. J. Perinatol. 2000, 20, S132–S137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schroeder, L.; Petrou, S.; Kennedy, C.; McCann, D.; Law, C.; Watkin, P.M.; Worsfold, S.; Yuen, H.M. The Economic Costs of Congenital Bilateral Permanent Childhood Hearing Impairment. Pediatrics 2006, 117, 1101–1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Chorozoglou, M.; Mahon, M.; Pimperton, H.; Worsfold, S.; Kennedy, C.R. Societal costs of permanent childhood hearing loss at teen age: A cross-sectional cohort follow-up study of universal newborn hearing screening. BMJ Paediatr. Open 2018, 2, e000228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Mehl, A.L.; Thomson, V. The Colorado Newborn Hearing Screening Project, 1992-1999: On the threshold of effective population-based universal newborn hearing screening. Pediatrics 2002, 109, e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Keren, R.; Helfand, M.; Homer, C.; McPhillips, H.; Lieu, T.A. Projected cost-effectiveness of statewide universal newborn hearing screening. Pediatrics 2002, 110, 855–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Kennedy, C.R.; Kimm, L.; Cafarelli Dees, D.; Campbell, M.J.; Thornton, A.R.D.; Bamber, J.; Innes, V.; Lloyd-Hughes, S.; Parish, R.; Woodhead, C.; et al. Controlled trial of universal neonatal screening for early identification of permanent childhood hearing impairment. Lancet 1998, 352, 1957–1964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Weichbold, V.; Welzl-Mueller, K. Maternal concern about positive test results in universal newborn hearing screening. Pediatrics 2001, 108, 1111–1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Tueller, S.J.; White, K.R. Maternal anxiety associated with newborn hearing screening. J. Early Hear. Detect. Interv. 2016, 1, 87–92. [Google Scholar]
  41. Watkin, P.M.; Baldwin, M.; Dixon, R.; Beckman, A. Maternal anxiety and attitudes to universal neonatal hearing screening. Br. J. Audiol. 1998, 32, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Crockett, R.; Wright, A.J.; Uus, K.; Bamford, J.; Marteau, T.M. Maternal anxiety following newborn hearing screening: The moderating role of knowledge. J. Med. Screen. 2006, 13, 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Crockett, R.; Baker, H.; Uus, K.; Bamford, J.; Marteau, T.M. Maternal anxiety and satisfaction following infant hearing screening: A comparison of the health visitor distraction test and newborn hearing screening. J. Med. Screen. 2005, 12, 78–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Vohr, B.R.; Letourneau, K.S.; McDermott, C. Maternal worry about neonatal hearing screening. J. Perinatol. 2001, 21, 15–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Kolski, C.; Le Driant, B.; Lorenzo, P.; Vandromme, L.; Strunski, V. Early hearing screening: What is the best strategy? Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol 2007, 71, 1055–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Khairi, M.D.M.; Rafidah, K.N.; Affizal, A.; Normastura, A.R.; Suzana, M.; Normani, Z.M. Anxiety of the Mothers with Referred Baby during Universal Newborn Hearing Screening. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol. 2011, 75, 513–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Stuart, A.; Moretz, M.; Yang, E.Y. An investigation of maternal stress after neonatal hearing screening. Am. J. Audiol. 2000, 9, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Tueller, S.J. Maternal Worry about Infant Health, Maternal Anxiety, and Maternal Perceptions of Child Vulnerability Associated with Newborn Hearing Screening Results. Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  49. Chiong, C.; Ostrea, E.J.; Reyes, A.; Llanes, E.G.; Uy, M.E.; Chan, A. Correlation of hearing screening with developmental outcomes in infants over a 2-year period. Acta Oto-Laryngologica 2007, 127, 384–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bevilacqua, M.C.; Alvarenga, K.d.F.; Costa, O.A.; Moret, A.L.M. The universal newborn hearing screening in Brazil: From identification to intervention. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2010, 74, 510–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Poonual, W.; Navacharoen, N.; Kangsanarak, J.; Namwongprom, S. Outcome of early identification and intervention on infants with hearing loss under universal hearing screening program. J. Med. Assoc. Thai. 2017, 100, 197. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  52. Sahli, A.S. Developments of children with hearing loss according to the age of diagnosis, amplification, and training in the early childhood period. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 2019, 276, 2457–2463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Olusanya, B.O.; Somefun, A.O. Place of Birth and Characteristics of Infants with Congenital and Early-Onset Hearing Loss in a Developing Country. Int. J. Ptry. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2009, 73, 1263–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. EQUATOR Network Guidelines. 2021. Available online: https://www.equator-network.org/reporting-guidelines/ (accessed on 1 May 2021).
  55. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Sedey, A.L.; Coulter, D.K.; Mehl, A.L. Language of Early- and Later-identified Children With Hearing Loss. Pediatrics 1998, 102, 1161–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Bishop, D.V.M. Test for Reception of Grammar; Age and Cognitive Performance Research Centre, University of Manchester: Manchester, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  57. Dunn, L.M.; Whetton, C.; Burley, J. British Picture Vocabulary Scale, 2nd ed.; NFER-Nelson: Windsor, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  58. Sparrow, S.S.; Balla, D.A.; Cicchetti, D.V. Vineland: Adaptive Behavior: Scales, Interview Edition: Survey form Manual; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  59. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions; The Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  60. Stothard, S.E.; Hulme, C.; Clarke, P.; Bowyer-Crane, C.; Harrington, A.; Truelove, E.; Nation, K. YARC York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension Secondary; GL Assessment: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  61. Ireton, H.; Thwing, E. The Minnesota Child Development Inventory in the psychiatric-developmental evaluation of the preschool-age child. Child. Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 1972, 3, 102–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Zimmerman, I.L.; Steiner, V.G.; Pond, R.E. Preschool Language Scale, 4th ed.; Harcourt Assessment: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  63. Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, D.M. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th ed.; Pearson: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  64. Renfrew, C. Renfrew Bus Story Manual: A Test of Narrative Speech, 3rd ed.; Winslow Press: Oxford, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  65. Bishop, D.V. Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 879–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Crystal, D.; Fletcher, P.; Garman, M. The Grammatical Analysis of Language Disability—A Procedure for Assessment and Remediation, 1st ed.; Edward Arnold: London, UK, 1976. [Google Scholar]
  67. Raven, C.; Raven, J.C.; Court, J.H. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales; Oxford Psychologists Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  68. Goodman, R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A Research Note. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 581–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Varni, J.W.; Burwinkle, T.M.; Seid, M.; Skarr, D. The PedsQL 4.0 as a pediatric population health measure: Feasibility, reliability, and validity. Ambul. Pediatr. 2003, 3, 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Fenson, L.; Marchman, V.A.; Thal, D.; Dale, P.S.; Bates, E.; Reznick, J.S. The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd ed.; Paul, H. Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  71. Sumner, G.; Spietz, A. NCAST Caregiver/Parent-Child Interaction Teaching Manual; NCAST/Caregiver: Seattle, WA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  72. Jerger, S.; Lewis, S.; Hawkins, J.; Jerger, J. Pediatric speech intelligibility test. I. Generation of test materials. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 1980, 2, 217–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Boothroyd, A.; Eisenberg, L.S.; Martinez, A.S. OLIMSPAC Version 3.1d; House Ear Institute: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  74. Fudala, J. Arizona Test of Articulation-3; PRO-Ed, Inc.: Austin, TX, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  75. Edwards, S.; Fletcher, P.; Garman, M.; Hughes, A.; Letts, C.; Sinda, I. Reynell Developmental Language Scales; NFER-Nelson Publishing: Windsor, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  76. Marteau, T.M.; Bekker, H. The development of a 6-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State Trait Anxiety In-ventory (STAI). Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1992, 31, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Montgomery, S.A.; Asberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiat. 1979, 134, 382–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Cox, J.L.; Holden, J.M.; Sagovsky, R. Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10 items Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br. J. Psychiat. 1987, 150, 782–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Beck, A.T.; Epstein, N.; Brown, G.; Steer, R.A. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988, 56, 893–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Abidin, R.R. The Parenting Stress Index: Manual, 3rd ed.; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. PRISMA systematic review flow diagram.
Figure 1. PRISMA systematic review flow diagram.
Jcm 10 02784 g001
Table 1. Search strategy used.
Table 1. Search strategy used.
Concept 1ANDConcept 2ANDConcept 3
(newborn hearing screening) OR (universal newborn hearing screening)(hearing loss) OR (hearing impairment) OR (childhood hearing impairment) OR (permanent childhood hearing loss)(outcome) OR (speech outcome) OR (language outcome) OR (literacy outcome) OR (maternal anxiety) OR (maternal stress)
Table 2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria using the Population, Intervention, Control, Outcomes, Study design and Timeframe (PICOST) criteria.
Table 2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria using the Population, Intervention, Control, Outcomes, Study design and Timeframe (PICOST) criteria.
InclusionExclusion
ParticipantsNewborns (gestational age ≥37 weeks at birth without complications) and children within the first year of life undergoing Newborn Hearing Screening (NHS)/ Universal Newborn Hearing Screening (UNHS)Only reporting screening of high-risk babies
InterventionsUniversal newborn hearing screeningTarget (or selective) screening with the UNHS with no comparison to UNHS
Comparators No screening or selected/targeted screen
Outcomes
Outcomes of UNHS in terms of Early Hearing Detection and Intervention (EHDI): age of identification, age of amplification, age of intervention start.
Developmental outcomes including receptive and expressive language, speech perception and speech production, literacy, social development, behavioral problems, quality of life.
Cost benefit or cost-effectiveness or cost estimates for UNHS if available
Adverse effects of UNHS on parents of children with hearing loss.
Not reporting the key outcomes listed
Study designs
Studies regardless of design including those that were observational, including cohorts, case-controls or cross-sectional.
Studies that include comparisons with controlled comparisons or that have done universal screening without controlled comparisons but have reported data for healthy and at-risk infants separately.
Must have had NHS in place or being tested on the population.
Must report the total number of children screened and the number of children with confirmed permanent bilateral hearing loss detected as a result of NHS or UNHS.
Must have reported the total number of children undergoing NHS or UNHS screening.
Reviews, comment pieces, letters or editorials
Study reporting on a selective sample at risk of hearing loss without comparison with UNHS or healthy infants.
Studies where the full spectrum of hearing loss was not represented.
Studies where NHS/UNHS was not in place or being tested.
Studies which did not specify the total number of children confirmed to have hearing loss at NHS/UNHS.
Studies which did not specify the total number of children undergoing NHS/UNHS or with numerator bias.
TimingsNo restrictions No restrictions
Other criteriaEnglish language articles Non-English articles
Table 3. Types of outcomes of UNHS reported in the studies included (n = 30).
Table 3. Types of outcomes of UNHS reported in the studies included (n = 30).
StudyEarly Identification and InterventionDevelopmental OutcomesCost AnalysisAdverse Effects
Age of IdentificationAge at AmplifyIntervention StartReceptive LanguageExpressive LanguageSpeech PerceptionSpeech ProductionLiteracySocial devBehavioral ProblemsQuality of Life
Question 1: Early identification and intervention (7 studies)
Kennedy et al. (2006) [19]
Uus and Bamford (2006) [20]
Wood et al. (2015) [21]
Wake et al. (2016) [22]
Sininger et al. (2009) [23]
Dalzell et al. (2000) [24]
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (2001) [25]
Question 2: Developemental outcomes (11 studies, Kennedy et al., 2006 and Wake et al., 2016 listed earlier)
McCann et al. (2008) [26]
Worsfold et al. (2010) [27]
Stevenson et al. (2010) [28]
Stevenson et al. (2018) [29]
Pimperton et al. (2016) [30]
Korver et al. (2010) [31]
Sininger et al. (2010) [32]
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (2000) [33]
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (2020) [11]
Question 3: Cost effectiveness
(4 studies)
Schroeder et al. (2006) [34]
Chorozoglou et al. (2018) [35]
Mehl and Thomson (2002) [36]
Keren et al. (2002) [37]
Question 4: Adverse effects
(10 studies)
Kennedy et al. (1998) [38]
Weichbold and Welzl-Mueller (2001) [39]
Tueller and White (2016) [40]
Watkin et al. (1998) [41]
Crockett et al. (2006) [42]
Crockett et al. (2005) [43]
Vohr et al. (2001) [44]
Kolski et al. (2007) [45]
Khairi et al. (2011) [46]
Stuart et al. (2000) [47]
Table 4. CASP ratings for included studies based on its design.
Table 4. CASP ratings for included studies based on its design.
a. CASP ratings for cohort studies (Y = Yes, C = Can’t tell, N = No, NA = Not applicable)
Article1. Did the study address a clearly focused issue?2. Was the cohort recruited in an acceptable way?3. Was the exposure accurately measured to minimize bias?4. Was the outcome accurately measured to minimize bias?5. (a) Have the authors identified all important confounding factors?5. (b) Have they taken account of the confounding factors in the design and/or analysis?6. (a) Was the follow up of subjects complete enough?6. (b) Was the follow up of subjects long enough?7. What are the results of this study?8. How precise are the results?9. Do you believe the results?10. Can the results be applied to the local population?11. Do the results of this study fit with other available evidence?12. What are the implications of this study for practice?
Kennedy et al. (2006) [19]YYYYCCYYYCYYYY
Uus and Bamford (2006) [20]YYYYYYYYYYYYYY
Wood et al. (2015) [21]YYYYYYYYYYYYYY
Wake et al. (2016) [22]YYYYYYYCYCYYYY
Sininger et al. (2010) [32]YCCYCCCYCYYYYY
Dalzell et al. (2000) [24]YYYYYCCCYYYYYY
McCann et al. (2008) [26]YYYYCCYYYYYYYY
Worsfold et al. (2010) [27]YYYYCCYYYCYYYY
Stevenson et al. (2010) [28]YYYYCCYYCCYYYY
Stevenson et al. (2018) [29]YYYYCCYYYYYYYY
Pimperton et al. (2016) [30]YYYYCCYYYYYYYY
Korver et al. (2010) [31]YYCCCCYYCCYCCC
Sininger et al. (2009) [23]YCCCCCCCYCYYYY
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (2020) [11]YYYYCCYYYYYYYY
Kennedy (1998) [38]YYYYCYYYYCYYYY
Weichbold and Welzl-Mueller (2001) [39]YYYCCCCYCCYYYY
Tueller and White (2016) [40] and Tueller (2006) [48]YYYYYYYYYYYYYY
Watkin et al. (1998) [41]YYYYCYYYYYYYYY
Crockett et al. (2006) [42]YYYCYYCCYYYYYY
Crockett et al. (2005) [43]YCYYCCYYYCYYYY
Vohr et al. (2001) [44]YYCCCCCCYYYYYY
Kolski et al. (2007) [45]YYYYCCYCYYYYYY
Khairi et al. (2011) [46]YYYCCCCCYCYYYY
Stuart at al. (2000) [47]YYYCCCYCYYYYYY
b. CASP ratings case-control studies (Y = Yes, C = Can’t tell, N = No, NA = Not applicable)
Article1. Did the study address a clearly focused issue?2. Did the authors use an appropriate method to answer their question?3. Were the cases recruited in an acceptable way?4. Were the controls selected in an acceptable way?5. Was the exposure accurately measured to minimize bias?6. (a) Aside from the experimental intervention, were the groups treated equally?6. (b) Have the authors taken account of the potential confounding factors in the design and/or in their analysis?7. How large was the treatment effect?8. How precise was the estimate of the treatment effect?9. Do you believe the results?10. Can the results be applied to the local population?11. Do the results of this study fit with other available evidence?
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (2001) [25]YYYYYCYYYYYY
Yoshinaga-Itano et al. (2000) [33]YYCCYYCYYYYY
c. CASP ratings economic evaluation studies (Y = Yes, C = Can’t tell, N = No, NA = Not applicable)
Article1. Was a well-defined question posed?2. Was a comprehensive description of the competing alternatives given?3. Does the paper provide evidence that the programme would be effective? (i.e., would the programme do more good than harm?)4. Were the effects of the intervention identified, measured and valued appropriately?5. Were all important and relevant resources required, and health outcome costs for each alternative identified, measured in appropriate units and valued credibly?6. Were costs and consequences adjusted for different times at which they occurred (discounting)?7. What were the results of the evaluation?their analysis?8. Was an incremental analysis of the consequences and cost of alternatives performed?9. Was an adequate sensitivity analysis performed?10. Is the programme likely to be equally effective in your context or setting?11. Are the costs translatable to your setting?12. Is it worth doing in your setting?
Schroeder et al. (2006) [34]YYYYYCYCNAYYY
Chorozoglou et al. (2018) [35]YYYYYCYCNAYYY
Mehl and Thomson (2002) [36]YYYCCCYCNYYY
Keren et al. (2002) [37]YYYYCNYYNAYCY
Table 5. GRADE level of evidence for UNHS outcomes.
Table 5. GRADE level of evidence for UNHS outcomes.
Certainty Assessment№ of PatientsEffectCertaintyImportance
№ of StudiesStudy DesignRisk of BiasInconsistencyIndirectnessImprecisionOther ConsiderationsUNHSNo Screen and/or Targeted ScreenRelative (95% CI)Absolute (95% CI)
Early identification and intervention (follow up: median 3 years)
7 observational studies not serious serious not serious not serious strong association
all outcomes indicate lower age ID, age amp, and age Rx
5,882,275596,874 not estimable NA⨁⨁◯◯ LOW CRITICAL
Developmental outcomes (follow up: median 3 years)
11 observational studies serious serious not serious serious strong association all plausible residual confounding would reduce the demonstrated effect 725,0371,556,737not estimable NA⨁◯◯◯ VERY LOW CRITICAL
Cost-benefit (follow up: median 5 years)
4 observational studies serious serious not serious not serious none 68,71488,019not estimable NA⨁◯◯◯ VERY LOW CRITICAL
Negative side effects (follow up: median 3 months)
10observational studies serious not serious not serious not serious none 717,82688,019not estimable NA⨁◯◯◯ VERY LOW IMPORTANT
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Manchaiah, V.; Hunnicutt, C. Outcomes of Universal Newborn Screening Programs: Systematic Review. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 2784. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132784

AMA Style

Yoshinaga-Itano C, Manchaiah V, Hunnicutt C. Outcomes of Universal Newborn Screening Programs: Systematic Review. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2021; 10(13):2784. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132784

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine, Vinaya Manchaiah, and Cynthia Hunnicutt. 2021. "Outcomes of Universal Newborn Screening Programs: Systematic Review" Journal of Clinical Medicine 10, no. 13: 2784. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132784

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop