Skip to main content
Log in

The Dominance of Associative Theorizing in Implicit Attitude Research: Propositional and Behavioral Alternatives

  • Theoretical Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the present article we re-examine one of the most deeply entrenched assumptions in modern attitude research, namely, that implicit social cognition is a product of associations between mental representations. More precisely, we argue that the analysis of implicit social cognition in psychology is curtailed by the widespread adoption of the associative assumption. We open with a brief overview of the attitude literature, with a particular emphasis on the fundamental structure, measurement, and conceptual differences that have emerged between implicit and explicit attitudes in recent times. Thereafter we address the influence of the associative assumption in shaping our methodologies, research questions, and theories regarding implicit and explicit attitudes. In the third and final section, we offer two alternative and perhaps complementary nonassociative models for understanding implicit cognition. While the first model situates its explanation at the mental (propositional) level of analysis and the second at the functional, each potentially allows for novel theoretical and empirical predictions and insight into attitudes above and beyond the boundaries of traditional associationism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ALBARRACIN, D., GLASMAN, L. R., & WALLACE, H. M. (2004). Survival and change in judgments: A model of activation and comparison. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 251–315). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ALBARRACIN, D., JOHNSON, B. T., & ZANNA, M. P. (2005). The handbook of attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • BANAJI, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, … A. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 117–150). Washington, Dc: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • BAR-ANAN, Y., NOSEK, B. A., & VIANELLO, M. (2009). The sorting paired features task: A measure of association strengths. Experimental Psychology, 56, 329–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BARDEN, J., MADDUX, W. W., PETTY, R. E., & BREWER, M. B. (2004). Contextual moderation of racial bias: The impact of social roles on controlled and automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & STEWART, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32, 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., Stewart, I. & BOLES, S. (2010). A sketch of the implicit relational assessment procedure (Irap) and the relational elaboration and coherence (Rec) model. The Psychological Record, 60, 527–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., MURPHY, A., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2010). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap): Exploring the impact of private versus public contexts and the response latency criterion on pro-White and anti-Black stereotyping among white Irish individuals. The Psychological Record, 60, 57–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., MURTAGH, L., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2010). Using the Implicit Association Test and the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure to measure attitudes towards meat and vegetables in vegetarians and meat-eaters. The Psychological Record, 60, 287–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., WALDRON, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2009). Testing the validity of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap) and the Implicit Association Test (Iat): Measuring attitudes towards Dublin and country life in Ireland. The Psychological Record, 59, 389–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • BIGLAN, A., & HAYES, S. C. (1996). Should the behavioral sciences become more pragmatic? The case for functional contextualism in research on human behavior. Applied and Preventive Psychology: Current Scientific Perspectives, 5, 47–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BLANTON, H., JACCARD, J., GONZALES, P. M., & CHRISTIE, C. (2006). Decoding the Implicit Association Test: Implications for criterion prediction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BROWN, A. S., GRAY, N. S., & SNOWDEN, R. J. (2009). Implicit measurement of sexual associations in child sex abusers. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21(2), 166–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • BRUNSTROM, J. M. (2007). Associative learning and the control of human dietary behavior. Appetite, 49(1), 268–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CHAN, G., BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2009). Implicit attitudes to work and leisure among North American and Irish individuals: A preliminary study. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 317–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • COLLINS, A. M., & LOFTUS, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CONREY, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & GROOM, C. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CRANO, W. D., & PRISLIN, R. (2006). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 345–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CRANO, W., & PRISLIN, R. (2008). Attitudes and attitude change. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • CRONBACH, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing (5th ed.). New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • CULLEN, C., BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2009). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap) and the malleability of ageist attitudes. The Psychological Record, 59, 591–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • DASGUPTA, N., & GREENWALD, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DAWSON, D. L., BARNES-HOLMES, D., Gresswell, D. M., Hart, A. J. P., & GORE, N. J. (2009). Assessing the implicit beliefs of sexual offenders using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: A first study. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 21, 57–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • DE HART, T., PELHAM, B.W., & TENNEN, H. (2006). What lies beneath: Parenting style and implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2003). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology 50, 77–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2006a). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In R.W. Wiers … A. W. Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2006b). Using the Implicit Association Test does not rule out an impact of conscious propositional knowledge on evaluative conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 37, 176–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2007). A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative conditioning. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 230–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2009a). Comparing measures of attitudes at the procedural and functional level. In R. Petty, R. H. Fazio, … P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 361–390). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2009b). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning and Behavior, 37, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J. (2011). Why the cognitive approach in psychology would profit from a functional approach and vice versa. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 202–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J., & MOORS, A. (2007). How to define and examine the implicitness of implicit measures. In B. Wittenbrink … N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and controversies (pp. 179–194). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J., & MOORS, A. (2010). Implicit measures: Similarities and differences. In B. Gawronski, … B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & MOORS, A. (2009a). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DE HOUWER, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & MOORS, A. (2009b). Theoretical claims necessitate basic research: Reply to Gawronski, Lebel, Peters, and Banse (2009) and Nosek and Greenwald (2009). Psychological Bulletin, 135, 377–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DEVINE, P. G., PLANT, E. A., AMODIO, D. M., HARMON-JONES, E., & VANCE, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 835–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DEVOS, T., & BANAJI, M. R. (2005). American = White? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 447–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DOVIDIO, J. F., KAWAKAMI, K., & GAERTNER, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 62–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DOVIDIO, J. F., KAWAKAMI, K., SMOAK, N., & GAERTNER, S. L. (2009). The roles of implicit and explicit processes in contemporary prejudice. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, … P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 165–192). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DOVIDIO, J. F., KAWAKAMI, K., JOHNSON, C., JOHNSON, B., & HOWARD, A. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DOTSCH, R., & WIGBOLDUS, D. H. J. (2008). Virtual prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1194–1198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EGLOFF, B., & SCHMUKLE, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ESSES, V. M., Dovidio, J. F., & HODSON, G. (2002). Public attitudes toward immigration in the United States and Canadian response to the September 11, 2001, ‘Attack on America.’ Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 2, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EVANS, J. S. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FAZIO, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The Mode model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FAZIO, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25, 603–637.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FAZIO, R. H., JACKSON, J. R., DUNTON, B. C., & WILLIAMS, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FAZIO, R., & OLSON, M. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FAZIO, R. H., & TOWLES-SCHWEN, T. (1999). The Mode Model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken … Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 97–116). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • FERGUSON, M. J. & BARGH, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 557–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FESTINGER, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • FIEDLER, K., MESSNER, C., & BLUEMKE, M. (2006). Unresolved problems with the “I”, the “A”, and the “T”: A logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test (Iat). European Review of Social Psychology, 17, 74–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FOX, E. (2006). Constructing a pragmatic science of learning and instruction with functional contextualism. Educational Technology Research & Development, 54(1), 5–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FRIESE, M., Hofmann, W., & WÄNKE, M. (2008). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 397–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GALDI, S., ARCURI, L., & GAWRONSKI, B. (2008). Automatic mental associations predict future choices of undecided decision makers. Science, 321, 1100–1102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GAWRONSKI, B. (2009). Ten frequently asked questions about implicit measures and their frequently supposed, but not entirely correct answers. Canadian Psychology, 50, 141–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAWRONSKI, B., & BODENHAUSEN, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GAWRONSKI, B., & BODENHAUSEN, G. V. (2007a). Unraveling the processes underlying evaluation: Attitudes from the perspective of the Ape Model. Social Cogniton, 25, 687–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAWRONSKI, B., & BODENHAUSEN, G. V. (2007b). What do we know about implicit attitude measures and what do we have to learn? In B. Wittenbrink … N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Progress and controversies (pp. 265–286). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • GAWRONSKI, B., & STRACK, F. (2004). On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 535–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., & BANAJI, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & MELLOTT, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, andself-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., & KRIEGER, L. H. (2006). Implicit bias: Scientific foundations. California Law Review, 94, 945–967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., MCGHEE, D. E., & SCHWARTZ, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREENWALD, A. G., NOSEK, B. A., BANAJI, M. R., & KLAUER, K. C. (2005). Validity of the salience asymmetry interpretation of the Iat: Comment on Rothermund and Wentura (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 420–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREGG, A. P., SEIBT, B., & BANAJI, M. H. (2006). Easier done than undone: Asymmetries in the malleability of implicit preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREY, I., & BARNES, D. (1996). Stimulus equivalence and attitudes. The Psychological Record, 46, 243–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • GROSS, A., & FOX, E. J. (2009). Relational frame theory: An overview of the controversy. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 25, 87–98

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GRUMM, M., NESTLER, S., & VON COLLANI, G. (2009). Changing explicit and implicit attitudes: The case of self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 327–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HALL, G. (2009). Learning in simple systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 210–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, Relational Frame Theory, and the third wave of behavior therapy. Behavior Therapy 35, 639–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & ROCHE, B. (Eds.). (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., LUOMA, J. B., BOND, F. W., MASUDA, A., & LILLIS, J. (2006). Acceptance and Commitment therapy: Model, processes, and outcomes. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HE, Y., JOHNSON, M. K., DOVIDIO, J. F., & MCCARTHY, G. (2009). The relation between race-related implicit associations and scalp-recorded neural activity evoked by faces from different races. Social Neuroscience, 4, 426–442.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HOLTGRAVES, T. (2004). Social desirability and self-reports: Testing models of socially desirable responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30, 161–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • HUGHES, S., & BARNES-HOLMES, D. (2011). On the formation and persistence of implicit attitudes: New evidence from the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap). The Psychological Record.

    Google Scholar 

  • HULL, C.L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • INBAR, Y., PIZARRO, D. A., KNOBE, J., & BLOOM, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9, 435–439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KAHNEMAN, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KARPINSKI, A., & STEINMAN, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KOOLE, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & VAN KNIPPENBERG, A. (2001). Whats in a name: Implicit self-esteem and the automatic self’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 669–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • KROSNICK, J. A., JUDD, C. M., & WITTENBRINK, B. (2005). The measurement of attitudes. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, … M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 21–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LIEBERMAN, M. D., GAUNT, R., GILBERT, D. T., & TROPE, Y. (2002). Reflection and reflexion: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 199–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LOVIBOND, P. F., & SHANKS, D. R., (2002). The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoretical interpretations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 28, 3–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MCCONNELL, A. R., & LEIBOLD, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MCCONNELL, A. R., RYDELL, R. J., STRAIN, L. M., & MACKIE, D. M. (2008). Forming implicit and explicit attitudes toward individuals: Social group association cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 792–807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MCKENNA, I. M., BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2007). Testing the fake-ability of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap): The first study. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 7, 253–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • MITCHELL, C. J., DE HOUWER, J., & LOVIBOND, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183–198

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • NISBETT, R. E., & WILSON, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NOSEK, B. A. (2007) Implicit-explicit relations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 65–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NOSEK, B. A., & BANAJI, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NOSEK, B., Banaji, M., & GREENWALD, A. (2002). Math=male, me=female, therefore math * me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 44–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’HORA, D., PELÂEZ, M., BARNES-HOLMES, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & CHAUDHARY, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the Wais-Iii. The Psychological Record, 58, 569–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • OLSON, M. A., & FAZIO, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude formation through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 12, 413–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • OLSON, M. A., & FAZIO, R. H. (2002). Implicit acquisition and manifestation of classically conditioned attitudes. Social Cognition, 20, 89–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OLSON, M. A., & FAZIO, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 421–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • OLSON, M. A., & FAZIO, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the Mode model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, … P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 19–63). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • OLSON, M. A., & KENDRICK, R. V. (2008). Origins of attitudes. In W. Crano … R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 111–131). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • PAULHUS, D. L. (1989). Socially desirable responding. In D. M. Buss … N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 201–209). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • PAVLOV, I. P. (1928). Twenty-five years of objective study of the higher nervous activity (behavior) of animals [W.H. Gantt, Trans.]. New York: International.

    Google Scholar 

  • PAYNE, B. K., CHENG, S. M., GOVORUN, O., & STEWART, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PAYNE, B. K., & GAWRONSKI, B. (2010). A history of implicit social cognition: Where is it coming from? Where is it now? Where is it going? In B. Gawronski … B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • PELHAM, B. W., MIRENBERG, M. C., & JONES, J. K. (2002). Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: Implicit egotism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 469–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PETTY, R. E., & BRINOL, P. (2006). A meta-cognitive approach to “implicit” and “explicit” evaluations: Comment on Gawronski and Bodenhausen (2006). Psychological Bulletin, 132, 740–744.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • POWER, P. M., BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2009). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap) as a measure of implicit relative preferences: A first study. The Psychological Record, 59, 621–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • RANGANATH, K. A., & NOSEK, B. A. (2008). Implicit attitude generalization occurs immediately, explicit attitude generalization takes time. Psychological Science, 19, 249–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • REHFELDT, R. A., & BARNES-HOLMES, Y. (2009). Derived relational responding: Applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities. Oakland, CA: Context Press/New Harbinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • RINCK, M., & BECKER, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 105–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ROTHERMUND, K., WENTURA, D., & DE HOUWER, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus-response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 482–495.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • RUDMAN, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 79–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RYDELL, R. J., & GAWRONSKI, B. (2009). I like you, I like you not: Understanding the formation of context dependent automatic evaluations. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1118–1152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RYDELL, R. J., & MCCONNELL, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: A systems of reasoning analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 995–1008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • RYDELL, R. J., MCCONNELL, A. R., MACKIE, D. M., & STRAIN, L. M. (2006). Of two minds: Forming and changing valence inconsistent attitudes. Psychological Science, 17, 954–958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SCHNABEL, K., BANSE, R., & ASENDORPF, J. B. (2006). Employing automatic approach and avoidance tendencies for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept: The Implicit Association Procedure (Iap). Experimental Psychology, 53, 69–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARZ, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in context. Social Cognition, 25(5), 638–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHWARZ, N. (2008). “Attitude measurement.” In W. Crano … R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 41–60). London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SHANKS, D. R. (2007). Associationism and cognition: Human contingency learning at 25. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 291–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SHANKS, D. R. (2010). Learning: from association to cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 1, 273–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SHERMAN, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., & CHASSIN, L. (2003). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects of context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 13–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • SLOMAN, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SMITH, E. R., & DE COSTER, J. (1999). Associative and rule-based processing: A connectionist interpretation of dual process models. In S. Chaiken … Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 323–336). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • SMITH, E. R., & DE COSTER, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SRIRAM, N., & GREENWALD, A. G. (2009). The Brief Implicit Association Test. Experimental Psychology, 56, 283–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • STACY, A. W., AMES, S. L., & GRENARD, J. L. (2007). Word associations tests of associative memory and implicit processes: Theoretical and assessment issues. In R.W. Wiers … A.W. Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 75–90). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • STANLEY, D., PHELPS, E., & BANAJI, M. (2008). The neural basis of implicit attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 164–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STRACK, F., & DEUTSCH, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • TEACHMAN, B. A., GAPINSKI, K. D., BROWNELL, K. D., RAWLINS, M., & JEYARAM, S. (2003). Demonstrations of implicit anti-fat bias: The impact of providing causal information and evoking empathy. Health Psychology, 22, 68–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • THORNDIKE, E. L. (1931). Human learning. New York: Century

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • UZIEL, L. (2010). Rethinking social desirability scales: From impression management to interpersonally orientated self-control. Perspectives of Psychological Science, 5, 243–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VAHEY, N. A., BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & STEWART, I. (2009). A first test of Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (Irap) as a measure of self-esteem: Irish prisoner groups and university students. The Psychological Record, 59, 371–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • VANMAN, E. J., SALTZ, J. L., NATHAN, L. R., & WARREN, J. A. (2004). Racial discrimination by low-prejudiced Whites. Psychological Science, 15, 711–719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VON HIPPEL, W., BRENER, L., & VON HIPPEL, C. (2008). Implicit prejudice toward injecting drug users predicts intentions to change jobs among drug and alcohol nurses. Psychological Science, 19, 7–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WATT, A. W., KEENAN, M., BARNES, D., & CAIRNS, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WEINSTEIN, J. H., WILSON, K. G., DRAKE, C. E., & KELLUM, K. K. (2008). A Relational Frame Theory Contribution to Social Categorization. Behavior and Social Issues, 17(1), 40–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WIGBOLDUS, D., HOLLAND, R. W., & VAN KNIPPENBERG, A. (2004). Single-target implicit associations. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • WILSON, T. D. (2009). Know thyself. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 384–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WILSON, T. D., LINDSEY, S., & SCHOOLER, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WILSON, T. D., & SCHOOLER, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WITTENBRINK, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. Wittenbrink … N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 17–58). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • WITTENBRINK, B., JUDD, C. M., & PARK, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship to questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WITTENBRINK, B., JUDD, C. M., & PARK, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 815–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean Hughes.

Additional information

This article is also published in Spanish as a chapter in M. Valverdre & M. Alvaerz (Eds.), Current perspectives in human learning. The work was completed while the first author was in receipt of a scholarship from the Irish Research Council for Science, Engineering and Technology (IRCSET).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hughes, S., Barnes-Holmes, D. & De Houwer, J. The Dominance of Associative Theorizing in Implicit Attitude Research: Propositional and Behavioral Alternatives. Psychol Rec 61, 465–496 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395772

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395772

Key words

Navigation