Next Article in Journal
Perceptions and Patterns of Cigarette and E-Cigarette Use among Hispanics: A Heterogeneity Analysis of the 2017–2019 Health Information National Trends Survey
Previous Article in Journal
Pregnancy Outcomes among Women with Intermittent Asthma: A Retrospective Cohort Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Long Working Hours and Night Work on Subjective Well-Being Depending on Work Creativity and Task Variety, and Occupation: The Role of Working-Time Mismatch, Variability, Shift Work, and Autonomy

1
Department of Medicine, Medical College, Pusan National University, Yangsan 50612, Korea
2
Department of Preventive, and Occupational & Environmental Medicine, Medical College, Pusan National University, Yangsan 50612, Korea
3
Environmental Health Center of Asbestos, Pusan National University Yangsan Hospital, Yangsan 50612, Korea
4
Medical Research Institute, Pusan National University, Yangsan 50612, Korea
5
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Pusan National University Yangsan Hospital, Yangsan 50612, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(12), 6371; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126371
Submission received: 6 April 2021 / Revised: 4 June 2021 / Accepted: 10 June 2021 / Published: 12 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Occupational Safety and Health)

Abstract

:
This study explored the effects of long working hours (LW) and night work (NW) on subjective well-being and the modifying effects of work creativity and task variety (WCTV) and occupation. In addition, we examined the influence of working time-related variables including working-time mismatch, variability, shift work, and autonomy on the effects of LW and NW. This study used data from the 5th Korean Working Conditions Survey on 50,205 workers. LW and NW were defined as 52–60 h (L1) or >60 h (L2) per week, and 1–10 days (N1) or >10 days (N2) of night work per month. Multiple logistic regression was used to examine the effects of LW and NW and the modifying influences of WCTV and occupation. Differences in ORs of LW and NW caused by working time-related variables were investigated, to determine effect sizes and directions. A high level of WCTV alleviated the risks of LW and NW. White-collar workers were more vulnerable to the risk associated with NW. Regarding working-time related variables, working-time mismatch and variability increased the risks of LW and NW, respectively, while shift work alleviated the risks of NW. In countries where flexible work systems are not well utilized, working-time autonomy might not be associated with the risk of LW or NW. This study showed that it is necessary to comprehensively consider the occupation and task characteristics of individual workers performing LW or NW. Further studies of the modifying effects of working time-related variables on LW and NW are needed.

1. Introduction

In 2015, 16% of European workers worked more than 48 h per week and 19% worked nights [1]. These atypical work types have negative impacts on health such as on cardiovascular disease [2], metabolic disturbance [3,4], mortality [5,6], and depression [7]. However, although the effects of long working hours (LW) and night work (NW) have been investigated, few have addressed the synergistic effect of LW and NW using national-scale data.
Many studies have indicated that individual and environmental factors could influence the impact of LW and NW. For example, evening type workers showed high tolerance on NW [8,9] and female workers were more vulnerable to the risks posed by overtime work [10]. However, the effects of task characteristics, as compared with individual or environmental characteristics, on subjective well-being have received relatively little attention, though task characteristics such as complexity have been associated with exhaustion among night workers [11]. Furthermore, studies that examined the effect of task characteristics have reported conflicting results [12,13], which demonstrates the need to determine the extent to which task characteristics influence the effects of LW and NW on subjective well-being [14]. In addition, it is also possible to infer that the effects of LW and NW vary by occupation, even if workers had the same task characteristics, based on a previous study which showed that the risk of LW varied by occupation type (manual vs. non-manual) [15]. Therefore, it is necessary to examine the effects of LW and NW based on considerations of task characteristics and occupation type (blue collar vs. white collar).
A previous study reported that the effect of LW on life satisfaction depends on working-time mismatch [16]. Another study showed that the risk posed by LW was combined with working-time autonomy [17]. Because prior studies on the negative impacts of working-time mismatch and autonomy on subjective well-being were limited, there is a need to determine whether these working time-related variables increase or reduce the risks associated with LW and NW, and to what extent these risks depend on task characteristics and occupation. In addition, the studies to date have mostly highlighted the negative effect of shift work in terms of night shift or working-time variability [7,18]. However, night and shift work are technically different concepts [19], which should be examined separately.
Therefore, this study was conducted to examine the effects, including the synergistic effects, of LW and NW on subjective well-being, and to determine how these effects depend on occupation and task characteristics among Korean workers. In addition, we examined the modifying effects of working time-related variables including working-time mismatch, variability, shift work, and autonomy on the effects of LW and NW with respect to work creativity and task variety (WCTV) and occupation.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study used data obtained from the 5th Korean Working Conditions Survey (KWCS) conducted in 2017. This survey was representative of the working environment in the Republic of Korea and included 50,205 workers the response rate was 0.449. A description of the sampling method used for the KWCS is described in the user guide report [20]. In KWCS, stratified random sampling was used to achieve nationwide representativeness. The present study included 50,205 economically active workers aged ≥15. Because participants did not respond to all questionnaire items, the numbers of samples included in the analyses differed slightly. The number of missing values for each variable among the 50,205 samples were as follows; (a) educational level (n = 52), (b) numbers of employee (n = 287), (c) subjective health conditions (n = 7), (d) monthly income (n = 4278), (e) working hours per week (n = 148), (f) WCTV (n = 156), (g) occupation (n = 93), (h) subjective well-being (n = 51), (i) working-time mismatch (n = 278), (j) working-time variability (n = 15), (k) shift work (n = 19), and (l) working-time autonomy (n = 111). All other variables had no missing values. All participants volunteered to participate in the KWCS and provided informed consent. Since KWCS derived data was public data from a national survey, IRB approval was not required.

2.2. Measurement

Subjective well-being was measured using the five-item World Health Organization Well-being Index. This index requires responses to five questions about overall feelings regarding cheerfulness, calmness, vigorousness, freshness, and interest over the previous two weeks [21]. Each response was scored using a six-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (none of the time) to 5 (all the time). The raw scores obtained (0 to 25 points) were transformed to scores from 0 (absence of well-being) to 100 points (maximal well-being) (Cronbach alpha = 0.925 in this study). The range 0 to 50 points was deemed low subjective well-being (LSW) and 51–100 as high. This dichotomization was performed for about 50 points because it has been previously used as a cut-off value for poor well-being [22]. WCTV variables were chosen to be in-line with the overview report of European Working Conditions Survey (EWCS) [1], and consisted of six components, that is, cognitive demand; (a) non-monotonicity, (b) non-repetitiveness, (c) complexity, (d) learning new things, (e) applying own ideas, and (f) solving unforeseen problems. Except for “applying own ideas”, which was a 5-point Likert scale, all components were rated using a dichotomous scale. Thus, the “applying own ideas” was set to 1 for “Always” or “Most of the time” and 0 for “Sometimes”, “Rarely”, or “Never”. The scores of these six components were combined to produce WCTV scores of 0–6 points (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.481 in this study). WCTV scores of 0–3 points were defined as low and scores of 4–6 as high. The KWCS also requested details of working hours. According to the Korean labor act, we defined 41–51 h per week as the standard and classified LW as 52–60 h (L1) or >60 h (L2). NW was defined as working more than 2 h between 10 p.m. and 5 a.m. In this case, the standard group was composed of individuals that did not work during the night over the previous month. NW was classified as 1–10 days per month (N1) and >10 days per month (N2). Occupations were classified as blue-collar or white-collar and according to the Korean Standard Classification of Occupations as; managers, professionals, clerks, service and sale workers (white-collar workers), and skilled agricultural, forestry and fishery workers, craft and related trades workers, plant, machine operators, and elementary occupations (blue-collar workers). About subjective health conditions, participants rated their overall subjective health conditions as one of “Very bad”, “Bad”, “Fair”, “Good”, and “Very good”. In this study, we classified them into “Bad”, “Fair”, and “Good”. Besides, sex, age, education level, number of employees, and monthly income were included in this study. Finally, we included four working time-related variables that might influence the effects of LW and NW on subjective well-being, that were; (a) working-time mismatch, (b) working-time variability, (c) shift work, and (d) working-time autonomy. Regarding working-time mismatch, we compared actual and desired working hours and classified them as “adequate”, “insufficient”, and “excessive”. For working-time autonomy, if scheduled work was fixed by the company or the worker could choose one of several schedules fixed by the company, the variable was set at “No”. On the other hand, if a worker could adapt his/her working hours within certain limits, or entirely determine the work schedule, the variable was set at “Yes”. The variability in working-time and shift work were expressed as yes or no.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

First, we conducted the Chi-square test and linear-by-linear association were used to identify trends of general characteristics for LW and NW. Second, the Chi-square test was used to determine whether the proportion of participants with LSW depended on LW or NW. When results were significant, we performed a pairwise comparison of proportions using Z-statistic. P-values were adjusted for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni’s correction method. Third, we conducted multiple logistic regression to examine the effects of LW and NW on subjective well-being and calculated Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% Confidence Intervals (CIs). Logistic regression analysis was performed on all study subjects and participants classified by WCTV and occupation. Fourth, we performed multiple logistic regression analysis adjusted for four variables that might influence the effects of LW and NW. Finally, we examined the difference in ORs of LW and NW for LSW from the model “after adjusted” for working time time-related variables to the model “not adjusted”, to investigate the effect sizes of variables and directions of changes. A positive value indicated that the variable increased the risks of LW or NW. A change was considered “substantial” when the ORs was changed by >10% or the statistical significance was changed. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 25.0 (IBM Crop., Armonk, NY, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Distribution of Demographic Variables According to LW and NW

The characteristics of the study group and distributions according to LW and NW are shown in Table 1. All variables showed significant results by the Chi-square and trend tests. Men performed more LW and NW, and workers in large companies had shorter working hours and performed more NW. In this study, workers with low WCTV performed more LW and NW. Blue-collar workers performed more NW but worked fewer hours than white-collar workers.

3.2. LW and NW According to WCTV and Occupation

Figure 1 shows the proportion of workers with LSW according to LW and NW, subdivided by WCTV and occupation. A higher proportion of blue-collar workers had LSW than white-collar, and a lower proportion of workers with high WCTV exhibited LSW. By occupation, blue-collar workers did not show a difference in LSW for both N1 and N2, as compared with reference (workers with no NW). In contrast, white-collar workers showed a higher proportion of LSW when they get N2. Figure 1 shows different aspects according to WCTV. Unlike the low WCTV groups, L1 did not exhibit a difference as compared with reference (workers with standard working hours) in the blue-collar high WCTV groups. In the blue-collar high WCTV groups, only L2 showed a relatively high proportion of LSW. The synergistic effect of LW and NW was not clear in this figure. In the low WCTV groups, only L2N2 showed a higher proportion of LSW as compared with the reference group (workers with standard working hours and no NW). For blue-collar workers with high WCTV, only L1N2 showed a difference in LSW. No difference was found for white-collar workers with high WCTV group.

3.3. Results of Multiple Logistic Regression for LSW

Table 2 shows the results of multiple logistic regression for LSW divided by occupation and WCTV. Regression analysis of all study subjects showed that all variables except for sex were related to LSW. Increases in LSW proportions were observed when workers performed LW (OR 1.18 for L1, OR 1.30 for L2) or NW (OR 1.17 for N1, OR 1.14 for N2). For WCTV and occupation, high WCTV (OR 0.87) and white-collar work (OR 0.82) were associated with lower LSW proportions. Regression analysis performed by occupation and WCTV showed LW generally increased LSW as compared to standard hours. L2 increased LSW regardless of occupation and WCTV, but L1 had a significant effect only in the low WCTV group. Regarding short working hours, for blue-collar workers, working less than the standard working hours had a negative effect on subjective well-being (significant for those with low WCTV), but a positive effect was observed for white-collar (significant for those with high WCTV). NW also had different effects on LSW according to occupation and WCTV. N1 had no significant effect on subjective well-being in the high WCTV groups. In the high WCTV blue-collar group, no NW of any kind increased or decreased LSW, and in the high WCTV white-collar group, only N2 considerably increased the proportion with LSW (OR 1.49). In the case of low WCTV groups, only N1 increased LSW (OR 1.22 for blue-collar, OR 1.28 for white-collar).
Table 3 shows the results of multiple logistic regression for LSW after adjusting for working time-related variables. Model 1 showed results adjusted for working-time mismatch. Regardless of occupation and WCTV, insufficient working hours increased LSW. In the case of excessive work, results showed a tendency toward LSW increase; only the low WCTV group showed a significant association. Model 2 produced results adjusted for working-time variability, which increased LSW proportions in all groups. In model 3, the effect of the shift generally decreased LSW, and this was significant in low WCTV groups. Model 4 showed working-time autonomy increased LSW in blue-collar workers with low WCTV and white-collar workers with high WCTV.
Figure 2 shows the OR differences for LW and NW from the model “after adjusted” for the variables shown in Table 3 to the model “not adjusted”. In the figure, a positive value means that the variable increased the risks of LW or NW. (A) and (B) show that the risk of LW and NW was generally reinforced, respectively, and (C) shows that the risk of NW was weakened. (A) shows that working-time mismatch influenced the risk of LW and that the risk of LW was more reinforced in low WCTV groups, a change in significance was only seen in L1 of blue-collar with low WCTV group. The influence of working-time variability is shown in (B). A change in the significance of NW was only observed in the low WCTV groups. (C) shows the influence of shift work on the effects of LW and NW. Similar to that shown in (B), OR differences were mainly observed for NW. However, unlike (B), shift work reduced the risk posed by NW, while variability in working times generally increased the risk of NW. Relatively little change in OR was caused by adjusting for working-time autonomy.

4. Discussion

The results of this study showed that LW and NW have negative effects on subjective well-being. Previous studies explained the negative effect of LW and NW on mental health by circadian rhythm disruption, sleep disturbance, lack of time to recover from work, and work-family conflicts [18,23,24,25]. Furthermore, the present study shows that creative and varied tasks have a positive effect on subjective well-being. Valcour explained that more complex tasks allowed workers autonomy and discretion, which promoted worker development such as in terms of skills and psychological resources [13]. The present study shows that working-time mismatch was found to be associated with the risks posed by LW, while variability of working time and shift work were associated with NW. However, working-time autonomy was less associated with LW or NW. This differs from a study conducted in the United States, which showed that control of work schedule enhanced work-life balance [26]. A previous study showed that the adoption rate of a flexible work system in Korea (12.5%) was considerably lower than that in the U.S. (81.0%) and Europe (66.0%) [27]. As a result, our observation could be attributed to the Korean labor culture in which the flexible work system was poorly utilized. Further study is needed to determine how the effects of autonomy with respect to working time depend on labor culture. We presented a theoretical framework based on the above results. On the left side, LW and NW act to decrease subjective well-being, and variables such as WCTV and occupation act to strengthen or weaken the effects. For example, high WCTV alleviated the risk of LW and NW and improved subjective well-being (Figure 3).
The synergistic effect of LW and NW was not demonstrated in this study, which contrasts with a prior study [28]. For blue-collar workers with high WCTV, a change in the LSW proportion was observed for L1N2 but not for L2N2. This could be interpreted as a result of a specificity of being blue-collar and being creative at the same time. In this study, desired working hours per week was longest in blue-collar workers with high WCTV. Furthermore, both enthusiasm at work and monthly income were higher than in the white-collar low WCTV group. A previous study showed that voluntary overtime work was less harmful than involuntary work, and rewards had a central role in the risk of overtime [29]. These findings suggest that the stress caused by simultaneous LW and NW might be relatively low in blue-collar workers with high WCTV.

4.1. LW and Subjective Well-Being

Although LW generally had a negative effect on subjective well-being, its effects depended on WCTV. L1 had a negative effect on subjective well-being in groups with a low WCTV, but had no significant effect for blue-collar workers with high WCTV (Figure 1). Moreover, the results of multiple logistic regression also showed that L1 did not affect LSW in groups with high WCTV, which suggested workers with high creativity work are less sensitive to LW. These results could be interpreted using model 1 in Table 3 and Figure 2A. Table 3 shows that the risk of LW was modified after adjusting for working-time mismatch, which concurs with a previous study [16]. Table 3 shows that subjective excessive work (when actual working hours were longer than wished) did not reduce subjective well-being in the high WCTV groups. Similarly, Figure 2A showed that the differences in the OR of LW after adjusting the working-time mismatch were relatively small in the high WCTV group. The above results suggested that individuals in the low WCTV groups were more sensitive to “working more than they wished,” indicating that even low-intensity LW could have an adverse effect on subjective well-being and that much of this adverse effect resulted from working-time mismatch.

4.2. NW and Subjective Well-Being

In this study, the effect of NW was affected by WCTV and occupation. In model 2 of Table 3, with the exception of the high WCTV white-collar group, NW did not have a significant effect on subjective well-being after adjusting for working-time variability. This result is attributed to a significant decrease in the OR of N1. In other words, it suggests that the risk of N1 could be due to unstable working hours rather than the fact that workers worked at night, as shown in Figure 2. Previous studies showed that working-time variability was an important cause of family conflict and caused workers to lose control of time schedules and personal plans [27,30]. The marked dependence of the risk of NW on working-time variability, also explains why N2 had a less detrimental effect on subjective well-being than N1 in Table 2, with the exception of high WCTV white-collar workers. Working more than 10 days on nights per month describes a normalized form of work, which had less harmful consequences than irregular NW. In fact, in this study, 45% of N2 workers worked at night almost every day of the month except weekends (>21 days per month) (data not shown).
When we adjusted for shift work, some interesting results were obtained. As shown in Figure 2, shift work resulted in a reduction in the LSW risk of NW, especially among blue-collar workers, suggesting that the reason why blue-collar workers were relatively insensitive to the risk of NW could be due to the effects of shift work. However, this result contrasts with those of previous studies, which reported shift work had negative effects on subjective well-being [31,32,33]. This study was able to infer the cause of these positive effects of shift work from the EWCS report. Workers in the health sector, one of the most representative areas for implementing shift in Europe, were most associated with working as a team (69%) [1]. Similarly, workers in electricity, gas, steam and water supply sectors, which accounted for most shift work in KWCS, also had the highest percentage of working as a team (48.5%). In addition, among night workers, shift workers (49.8%) were found to be much more team-based than non-shift workers (20.7%) in this study (Appendix A). For night workers, working as a team or alone might be an important factor of subjective well-being. In stressful situations, many studies have shown that social support from coworkers alleviates the negative effects of stress [34,35].
In addition, the unique characteristic of shift work could explain how the negative effects of NW were alleviated, given that the purpose of shift work often differs from that of other workers. For example, shift workers are often employed to maintain certain conditions (e.g., patient health or the freshness of goods) rather than to achieve performance-orientated goals. Therefore, shift workers work at speed to perform monotonous tasks [1], which is also shown by Appendix A. In the present study, these low WCTV-like characteristics were found to negatively affect subjective well-being. However, this is likely to have different effects on night workers, because, during NW, these monotonous task characteristics could alleviate the symptoms of worker exhaustion [36]. In fact, in Appendix A, the entire sample showed that shift workers (24.1%) reported higher levels of exhaustion than non-shift workers (21.4%). However, among night workers, the exhaustion level was lower in shift workers (22.4%) than in non-shift workers (24.1%). Summarizing, this study shows the above characteristics of shift work (team-based and low WCTV-like work) could alleviate the risk of NW on subjective well-being. Nonetheless, additional studies are required to determine the inherent characteristics of shift work.

4.3. Limitations

The first limitation of this study concerns the possibility of generalization. The results of this study are representative of Korean workers, but may not be representative of other nationalities. For example, our results concerning working-time autonomy conflicted with results obtained in a US study [26]. Furthermore, because labor cultures differ between countries, it may be difficult to apply our results equitably. Second, there is a problem of misclassification, as variables such as LW and NW were assessed by participant recall. In addition, the division of subjective well-being variables about cut-off values may have been an oversimplification, although it allowed us to use multiple logistic regression analysis to better understand the extents to which variables affected subjective well-being. In addition, previous studies have verified the validity of using 50 points as a cut-off value for poor well-being [22]. Third, researcher or self-response bias may have influenced data collection. However, the KWCS data collected during a national-scale survey did not focus on LW, NW, or subjective well-being. Thus, the risks of intentional and unintentional biases were relatively low. Fourth, we could not include variables such as marital status and self-efficacy, which reportedly have impacts on well-being [37]. Marital status could not be included in our study because the KWCS only contained information about family members living together and did not detail separated families or bereavements. The inclusion of such data may have strengthened or weakened relations identified [38,39]. Fifth, since responses to only one of the six questions were rated using a 5-point Likert scale in the process of constructing WCTV variable, information loss may have occurred when converting data to a dichotomous scale. In addition, WCTV had a relatively low Cronbach’s alpha value (0.481). This limitation was due to the lack of objective tools to measure WCTV in the data set. Consequently, additional research using validated measurement tools such as Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ) [40] and KEYS [41] is required. However, the composition of WCTV was similar to the decision latitude scale of the Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ) [42] and the Demand Control Support Questionnaire (DCSQ) [43], and the results of previous studies related to WCTV were similar to those obtained in the present study [44,45], which means that our WCTV measurements were largely valid. In addition, a prior study reported that the cut-off value for an acceptable and sufficient Cronbach’s alpha value is 0.45 [46]. Sixth, a change in statistical significance or OR of >10% were used as criteria of a “substantial” change. This was an operational definition rather than an absolute definition because it had no mathematical or statistical basis. Seventh, the statistical significance in this study might have been high, even if the effect of the variables was not heavy in practice, because the data we currently use had large samples. Finally, due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, we could not access the causalities of relationships. We suggest a longitudinal study be conducted to address these shortcomings. Nevertheless, this study is meaningful as it identifies the modulating effects of occupational characteristics and WCTV on relationships between subjective well-being, LW and NW.

5. Conclusions

Both LW and NW had negative effects on subjective well-being. High level of WCTV alleviated both the risk of LW and NW. White-collar workers were more vulnerable to the risk associated with NW as compared to blue-collar workers. Accordingly, it is necessary to comprehensively consider the occupation and task characteristics of workers performing LW or NW. In addition, working-time mismatch and variability increase the risk of LW and NW, respectively, and thus, workers’ desired working hours need to be reflected in working-time arrangements (especially in low WCTV workers), and NW needs to be adjusted to be as regular as possible. Performing shift work could alleviate the risks of NW. In countries where flexible work systems are not well utilized, as in Korea, working-time autonomy might not be associated with the risk of LW or NW. Further studies on the modifying effects of working time-related variables on LW and NW are needed.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.-G.S. and D.K.; methodology, M.-G.S. and D.K.; software, M.-G.S., Y.-J.K. and T.-K.K.; formal analysis, M.-G.S. and T.-K.K.; investigation, M.-G.S.; resources, M.-G.S. and Y.-J.K.; validation, D.K.; data curation, M.-G.S. and Y.-J.K.; writing—original draft preparation, M.-G.S. and T.K.K.; writing—review and editing, Y.-J.K. and D.K.; visualization, M.-G.S. and T.-K.K.; supervision, D.K.; project administration, Y.-J.K. and D.K.; funding acquisition, D.K.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Korea government (MSIT) (Grant no. NRF-2019R1A2C2084222). The funders were not involved in the study design, analysis, interpretation of data, writing, or submission of this manuscript. The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The raw KWCS data fo published paper used in this study is available from reference [20]. The data of report is available from the Occupational Safety and Health Research Institute (OSHIR). However, this data available with the approbal of the OSHIR.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) of Korea titled “Development of precision public health system to prevent metabolic syndrome of workers”.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Distribution of variables according to shift work among all study subjects and night workers.
Table A1. Distribution of variables according to shift work among all study subjects and night workers.
VariablesEntire Samplep-ValueWorkers with Night Workp-Value
No ShiftShiftNo ShiftShift
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
Work as team or group Yes10,146 (22.1)1792 (42.3)***545 (20.7)946 (49.8)***
No35,668 (77.9)2445 (57.7)2084 (79.3)952 (50.2)
Work creativity and task varietyHigh15,855 (34.6)980 (23.2)***851 (32.3)502 (26.6)***
Low29,954 (65.4)3250 (76.8)1782 (67.7)1388 (73.4)
Exhaustion after work High9811 (21.4)1023 (24.1)***688 (26.1)425 (22.4)**
Low36,026 (78.6)3218 (75.9)1951 (73.9)1472 (77.6)
p-values were determined using the Chi-square test. The question used to address “work as a team or group” was “Do you work in a group or team that has common tasks and can plan its work?”. The question used to address “exhaustion after work” was “ How often do you feel exhausted at the end of the working day?”. Workers who answered “always” or “most of the time” were considered at ‘high’ risk of exhaustion after work, and workers who answered “sometimes”, “rarely”, “never” were considered at ‘low’ risk. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

References

  1. Parent-Thirion, A.; Biletta, I.; Cabrita, J.; Vargas, O.; Vermeylen, G.; Wilczynska, A.; Wilkens, M. Sixth European Working Conditions Survey: Overview Report; Eurofound: Dublin, Ireland, 2016; pp. 1–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bøggild, H.; Knutsson, A. Shift work, risk factors and cardiovascular disease. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 1999, 25, 85–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Nakamura, K.; Shimai, S.; Kikuchi, S.; Takahashi, H.; Tanaka, M.; Nakano, S.; Motohashi, Y.; Nakadaira, H.; Yamamoto, M. Increases in body mass index and waist circumference as outcomes of working overtime. Occup. Med. 1998, 48, 169–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Antunes, L.C.; Levandovski, R.; Dantas, G.; Caumo, W.; Hidalgo, M.P. Obesity and shift work: Chronobiological aspects. Nutr. Res. Rev. 2010, 23, 155–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Åkerstedt, T.; Narusyte, J.; Svedberg, P. Night work, mortality, and the link to occupational group and sex. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2020, 46, 508–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Lee, H.-E.; Kim, I.; Kim, H.-R.; Kawachi, I. Association of long working hours with accidents and suicide mortality in Korea. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2020, 46, 480–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lee, A.; Myung, S.-K.; Cho, J.J.; Jung, Y.-J.; Yoon, J.L.; Kim, M.Y. Night Shift Work and Risk of Depression: Meta-analysis of Observational Studies. J. Korean Med Sci. 2017, 32, 1091–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Khaleque, A. Sleep Deficiency and Quality of Life of Shift Workers. Soc. Indic. Res. 1999, 46, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Smith, L.; Tanigawa, T.; Takahashi, M.; Mutou, K.; Tachibana, N.; Kage, Y.; Iso, H. Shiftwork Locus of Control, Situational and Behavioural Effects on Sleepiness and Fatigue in Shiftworkers. Ind. Health 2005, 43, 151–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Artazcoz, L.; Vera, A.G. Gender differences in the relationship between long working hours and health status in Catalonia. Arch. Prev. Riesgos Labor. 2012, 15, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. García-Sierra, R.; Fernández-Castro, J.; Martínez-Zaragoza, F. Relationship between job demand and burnout in nurses: Does it depend on work engagement? J. Nurs. Manag. 2016, 24, 780–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ng, T.W.H.; Feldman, D.C. Long work hours: A social identity perspective on meta-analysis data. J. Organ. Behav. 2008, 29, 853–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Valcour, M. Work-based resources as moderators of the relationship between work hours and satisfaction with work-family balance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2007, 92, 1512–1523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ganster, D.C.; Rosen, C.C.; Fisher, G.G. Long working hours and well-being: What we know, what we do not know, and what we need to know. J. Bus. Psychol. 2018, 33, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kim, B.H.; Lee, H.-E. The association between working hours and sleep disturbances according to occupation and gender. Chronobiol. Int. 2015, 32, 1109–1114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Wooden, M.; Warren, D.; Drago, R. Working Time Mismatch and Subjective Well-being. Br. J. Ind. Relat. 2009, 47, 147–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Bohle, P.; Quinlan, M.; Kennedy, D.; Williamson, A. Working hours, work-life conflict and health in precarious and “permanent” employment. Rev. Saude Publica 2004, 38, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Angerer, P.; Schmook, R.; Elfantel, I.; Li, J. Night Work and the Risk of Depression: A Systematic Review. Dtsch. Aerzteblatt Online 2017, 114, 404–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Härmä, M.; Karhula, K.; Puttonen, S.; Ropponen, A.; Koskinen, A.; Ojajärvi, A.; Kivimäki, M. Shift work with and without night work as a risk factor for fatigue and changes in sleep length: A cohort study with linkage to records on daily working hours. J. Sleep Res. 2019, 28, e12658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ko, J. User Guide of Raw Data for the 5th Korean Working Conditions Survey. Ulsan, Korea. 2017. Available online: https://oshri.kosha.or.kr/eoshri/resources/KWCSDownload.do (accessed on 13 May 2021).
  21. World Health Organization. Wellbeing Measures in Primary Health Care/the DEPCARE Project: Report on a WHO Meeting, Stockholm, Sweden, 12–13 February 1998. Available online: https://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0016/130750/E60246.pdf (accessed on 1 February 2021).
  22. Topp, C.W.; Østergaard, S.D.; Søndergaard, S.; Bech, P. The WHO-5 Well-Being Index: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Psychother. Psychosom. 2015, 84, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Neil-Sztramko, S.E.; Pahwa, M.; A Demers, P.; Gotay, C.C. Health-related interventions among night shift workers: A critical review of the literature. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2014, 40, 543–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. James, S.M.; Honn, K.A.; Gaddameedhi, S.; Van Dongen, H.P. Shift Work: Disrupted Circadian Rhythms and Sleep—Implications for Health and Well-being. Curr. Sleep Med. Rep. 2017, 3, 104–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Li, Z.; Dai, J.; Wu, N.; Jia, Y.; Gao, J.; Fu, H. Effect of long working hours on depression and mental well-being among employees in Shanghai: The role of having leisure hobbies. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Tausig, M.; Fenwick, R. Unbinding Time: Alternate Work Schedules and Work-Life Balance. J. Fam. Econ. Issues 2001, 22, 101–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. KCCI. Survey on Introduction of Flexible Work Arrangements in Company; Korea Chamber of Commerce and Industry Publishing: Seoul, Korea, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  28. Son, M.; Kong, J.-O.; Koh, S.-B.; Kim, J.; Härmä, M. Effects of long working hours and the night shift on severe sleepiness among workers with 12-hour shift systems for 5 to 7 consecutive days in the automobile factories of Korea. J. Sleep Res. 2008, 17, 385–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Beckers, D.G.; Van Der Linden, D.; Smulders, P.G.; Kompier, M.A.; Taris, T.; Geurts, S.A. Voluntary or involuntary? Control over overtime and rewards for overtime in relation to fatigue and work satisfaction. Work. Stress 2008, 22, 33–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Costa, G.; Sartori, S.; Åkerstedt, T. Influence of flexibility and variability of working hours on health and well-being. Chronobiol. Int. 2006, 23, 1125–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Moon, H.J.; Lee, S.H.; Lee, H.S.; Lee, K.-J.; Kim, J.J. The association between shift work and depression in hotel workers. Ann. Occup. Environ. Med. 2015, 27, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Nena, E.; Katsaouni, M.; Steiropoulos, P.; Theodorou, E.; Constantinidis, T.C.; Tripsianis, G. Effect of shift work on sleep, health, and quality of life of health-care workers. Indian J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2018, 22, 29–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Brown, J.P.; Martin, D.; Nagaria, Z.; Verceles, A.C.; Jobe, S.L.; Wickwire, E.M. Mental Health Consequences of Shift Work: An Updated Review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2020, 22, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Blanco-Donoso, L.M.; Garrosa, E.; Moreno-Jiménez, B.; De Almeida, E.C.; Villela-Bueno, S.M. Social Job Resources as Sources of Meaningfulness and its Effects on Nurses’ Vigor and Emotional Exhaustion: A Cross-Sectional Study Among Spanish Nurses. Curr. Psychol. 2017, 36, 755–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Velando-Soriano, A.; Ortega-Campos, E.; Gómez-Urquiza, J.L.; Ramírez-Baena, L.; De La Fuente, E.I.; La Fuente, G.A.C. Impact of social support in preventing burnout syndrome in nurses: A systematic review. Jpn. J. Nurs. Sci. 2020, 17, e12269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Cheng, W.-J.; Cheng, Y. Night shift and rotating shift in association with sleep problems, burnout and minor mental disorder in male and female employees. Occup. Environ. Med. 2017, 74, 483–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Helliwell, J.F. How’s life? Combining individual and national variables to explain subjective well-being. Econ. Model. 2003, 20, 331–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Richter, A.; Schraml, K.; Leineweber, C. Work–family conflict, emotional exhaustion and performance-based self-esteem: Reciprocal relationships. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2015, 88, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Wang, Z.; Liu, H.; Yu, H.; Wu, Y.; Chang, S.; Wang, L. Associations between occupational stress, burnout and well-being among manufacturing workers: Mediating roles of psychological capital and self-esteem. BMC Psychiatry 2017, 17, 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Morgeson, F.P.; Humphrey, S.E. The Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ): Developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design and the nature of work. J. Appl. Psychol. 2006, 91, 1321–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Amabile, T.M.; Conti, R.; Coon, H.; Lazenby, J.; Herron, M. Assessing the Work Environment for Creativity. Acad. Manag. J. 1996, 39, 1154–1184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Karasek, R.; Brisson, C.; Kawakami, N.; Houtman, I.; Bongers, P.; Amick, B. The Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ): An instrument for internationally comparative assessments of psychosocial job characteristics. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1998, 3, 322–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Mauss, D.; Herr, R.M.; Theorell, T.; Angerer, P.; Li, J. Validating the demand control support questionnaire among white-collar employees in Switzerland and the United States. J. Occup. Med. Toxicol. 2018, 13, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Conner, T.S.; Deyoung, C.G.; Silvia, P.J. Everyday creative activity as a path to flourishing. J. Posit. Psychol. 2018, 13, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Rasulzada, F.; Dackert, I. Organizational creativity and innovation in relation to psychological well-being and organizational factors. Creat. Res. J. 2009, 21, 191–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Taber, K.S. The use of Cronbach’s Alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ. 2018, 48, 1273–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Proportions of those with low subjective well-being according to long working hours and night work, subdivided by work creativity and task variety (WCTV) and occupation (A) blue collar workers, (B) white collar workers). Note: L1 indicates 52–60 h per week, L2 indicates >60 h per week, N1 indicates night work for 1–10 days per month, and N2 indicates night work for >10 days per month (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 for pairwise comparison and  p < 0.05,  p < 0.01, § p < 0.001 for chi-square test).
Figure 1. Proportions of those with low subjective well-being according to long working hours and night work, subdivided by work creativity and task variety (WCTV) and occupation (A) blue collar workers, (B) white collar workers). Note: L1 indicates 52–60 h per week, L2 indicates >60 h per week, N1 indicates night work for 1–10 days per month, and N2 indicates night work for >10 days per month (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 for pairwise comparison and  p < 0.05,  p < 0.01, § p < 0.001 for chi-square test).
Ijerph 18 06371 g001
Figure 2. Differences in the odds ratios of long working hours and night work on low subjective well-being from a model adjusted for (A) working-time mismatch, (B) working-time variability, (C) shift work, and (D) working-time autonomy to the model not adjusted. L1 indicates 52–60 h per week, L2 indicates >60 h per week, N1 indicates night work for 1–10 days per month, and N2 indicates night work for >10 days per month. A change was considered “substantial” when the ORs changed by >10% or the statistical significance changed (†: A change in statistical significance, WCTV: work creativity and task variety). Odds ratio adjusted for all other confounders (sex, age, education level, numbers of employee, subjective health condition, and monthly income).
Figure 2. Differences in the odds ratios of long working hours and night work on low subjective well-being from a model adjusted for (A) working-time mismatch, (B) working-time variability, (C) shift work, and (D) working-time autonomy to the model not adjusted. L1 indicates 52–60 h per week, L2 indicates >60 h per week, N1 indicates night work for 1–10 days per month, and N2 indicates night work for >10 days per month. A change was considered “substantial” when the ORs changed by >10% or the statistical significance changed (†: A change in statistical significance, WCTV: work creativity and task variety). Odds ratio adjusted for all other confounders (sex, age, education level, numbers of employee, subjective health condition, and monthly income).
Ijerph 18 06371 g002
Figure 3. Theoretical framework of relationship between subjective well-being, long working hours, and night work.
Figure 3. Theoretical framework of relationship between subjective well-being, long working hours, and night work.
Ijerph 18 06371 g003
Table 1. Distributions of demographic variables according to working hours per week and night work.
Table 1. Distributions of demographic variables according to working hours per week and night work.
Variablesn%Working Hours per WeekChi-Square Test (Trend Test)Night Work per MonthChi-Square Test (Trend Test)
<4141–5152–60>600 Day1–10 DaysOver 10 Days
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
SexMale23,70747.210,483
(44.4)
6653
(28.1)
4289
(18.1)
2210
(9.4)
***
(***)
20,660
(87.1)
1673
(7.1)
1374
(5.8)
***
(***)
Female26,49852.813,772
(52.1)
6340
(24.0)
4458
(16.9)
1852
(7.0)
25,004
(94.4)
761
(2.9)
733
(2.8)
Age<3912,95425.86967
(53.9)
3580
(27.7)
1822
(14.1)
555
(4.3)
***
(***)
11,754
(90.7)
769
(5.9)
431
(3.3)
***
(*)
40–4911,79523.55620
(47.8)
3269
(27.8)
2110
(17.9)
766
(6.5)
10,750
(91.1)
604
(5.1)
441
(3.7)
50–5913,25026.45322
(40.3)
3608
(27.3)
2883
(21.8)
1399
(10.6)
12,001
(90.6)
608
(4.6)
641
(4.8)
> 5912,20624.36346
(52.2)
2536
(20.9)
1932
(15.9)
1342
(11.0)
11,159
(91.4)
453
(3.7)
594
(4.9)
Educational levelUnder high school975119.45440
(56.0)
1936
(19.9)
1357
(14.0)
978
(10.1)
***
(***)
9112
(93.4)
277
(2.8)
362
(3.7)
***
(**)
High school19,15638.27102
(37.2)
5276
(27.6)
4573
(23.9)
2143
(11.2)
17,004
(88.8)
997
(5.2)
1155
(6.0)
Bachelor’s degree20,60041.111,217
(54.6)
5658
(27.5)
2762
(13.4)
921
(4.5)
18,909
(91.8)
1113
(5.4)
578
(2.8)
Masters or more6461.3469
(73.1)
112
(17.4)
45
(7.0)
16
(2.5)
591
(91.5)
43
(6.7)
12
(1.9)
Numbers of employee112,86325.84258
(33.2)
3071
(24.0)
3748
(29.2)
1740
(13.6)
***
(***)
11,867
(92.3)
427
(3.3)
569
(4.4)
***
(***)
2–919,90939.98689
(43.8)
5595
(28.2)
3732
(18.8)
1829
(9.2)
18,256
(91.7)
659
(3.3)
994
(5.0)
10–4910,25520.56437
(62.9)
2698
(26.4)
791
(7.7)
311
(3.0)
9325
(90.9)
605
(5.9)
325
(3.2)
50–24944658.92976
(66.7)
1049
(23.5)
309
(6.9)
126
(2.8)
3952
(88.5)
373
(8.4)
140
(3.1)
Over 24924264.91694
(69.9)
529
(21.8)
154
(6.4)
45
(1.9)
2004
(82.6)
357
(14.7)
65
(2.7)
Subjective health conditionGood33,05865.916,263
(49.3)
8706
(26.4)
5714
(17.3)
2292
(7.0)
***
(***)
30,169
(91.3)
1643
(5.0)
1246
(3.8)
***
(***)
Fair14,47128.86463
(44.8)
3822
(26.5)
2671
(18.5)
1470
(10.2)
13,015
(89.9)
710
(4.9)
746
(5.2)
Bad26695.31527
(57.6)
462
(17.4)
361
(13.6)
300
(11.3)
2474
(92.7)
81
(3.0)
114
(4.3)
Monthly income< $150113,68829.88647
(63.3)
2691
(19.7)
1446
(10.6)
887
(6.5)
***
(***)
12,771
(93.3)
393
(2.9)
524
(3.8)
***
(***)
$1501–$290021,62247.18541
(39.6)
6509
(30.2)
4598
(21.3)
1938
(9.0)
19,576
(90.5)
1111
(5.1)
935
(4.3)
$2901–$3800619113.52765
(44.7)
1741
(28.1)
1222
(19.8)
458
(7.4)
5512
(89.0)
431
(7.0)
248
(4.0)
> $380044269.62002
(45.3)
1144
(25.9)
841
(19.0)
433
(9.8)
3846
(86.9)
353
(8.0)
227
(5.1)
Work creativity and task varietyLow33,21066.415,887
(48.0)
8487
(25.6)
5722
(17.3)
3003
(9.1)
***
(***)
30,040
(90.5)
1453
(4.4)
1717
(5.2)
***
(***)
High16,83933.68293
(49.3)
4474
(26.6)
2999
(17.8)
1048
(6.2)
15,486
(92.0)
969
(5.8)
384
(2.3)
OccupationBlue collar18,98737.99461
(50.0)
5008
(26.5)
3007
(15.9)
1439
(7.6)
***
(***)
17,098
(90.1)
1048
(5.5)
841
(4.4)
***
(***)
White collar31,12562.114,728
(47.4)
7963
(25.6)
5736
(18.5)
2623
(8.4)
28,499
(91.6)
1360
(4.4)
1266
(4.1)
Subjective well-beingHigh34,38468.616,727
(48.7)
9194
(26.8)
5904
(17.2)
2500
(7.3)
***
(***)
31,447
(91.5)
1625
(4.7)
1312
(3.8)
***
(***)
Low15,77031.47510
(47.8)
3790
(24.1)
2836
(18.1)
1559
(9.9)
14,170
(89.9)
806
(5.1)
794
(5.0)
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Results of multiple logistic regression related to low subjective well-being according to work creativity and task variety, and occupation. Odds ratio (95% Confidence Interval).
Table 2. Results of multiple logistic regression related to low subjective well-being according to work creativity and task variety, and occupation. Odds ratio (95% Confidence Interval).
CovariatesAll
(n = 45,467)
Blue Collar WorkerWhite Collar Worker
Work Creativity and Task Variety
Low
(n = 12,761)
High
(n = 4150)
Low
(n = 17,268)
High
(n = 11,288)
SexMaleRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
Female0.96 (0.91–1.01)0.89 (0.82–0.97) **0.78 (0.65–0.94) *1.05 (0.97–1.14)0.96 (0.87–1.06)
Age<39Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
40–491.17 (1.10–1.25) ***1.20 (1.03–1.41) *0.94 (0.74–1.18)1.15 (1.05–1.27) **1.22 (1.09–1.37) ***
50–591.12 (1.04–1.19) **1.03 (0.89–1.20)0.97 (0.77–1.22)1.21 (1.09–1.34) ***1.04 (0.91–1.19)
>591.10 (1.02–1.20) *0.99 (0.85–1.16)0.95 (0.73–1.24)1.25 (1.09–1.43) **0.90 (0.73–1.11)
Educational levelUnder high schoolRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
High school0.73 (0.68–0.79) ***0.75 (0.68–0.83) ***0.98 (0.79–1.22)0.70 (0.61–0.80) ***0.64 (0.49–0.84) **
Bachelor’s degree0.66 (0.60–0.72) ***0.65 (0.56–0.76) ***0.93 (0.71–1.20)0.61 (0.53–0.71) ***0.59 (0.44–0.78) ***
Masters or more0.70 (0.56–0.88) **0.35 (0.10–1.27)2.21 (0.60–8.13)0.85 (0.57–1.27)0.55 (0.38–0.80) **
Numbers of employee1Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
2–91.01 (0.96–1.07)1.06 (0.97–1.16)0.93 (0.78–1.10)1.04 (0.95–1.14)1.03 (0.90–1.18)
10–491.04 (0.97–1.11)0.93 (0.83–1.04)0.73 (0.58–0.91) **1.24 (1.11–1.39) ***1.14 (0.98–1.34)
50–2491.04 (0.95–1.13)0.96 (0.82–1.12)0.79 (0.59–1.06)1.07 (0.92–1.26)1.24 (1.03–1.48) *
Over 2491.16 (1.04–1.30) **0.98 (0.79–1.21)0.86 (0.61–1.20)1.09 (0.87–1.37)1.56 (1.27–1.92) ***
Subjective health conditionGoodRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
Fair2.19 (2.09–2.29) ***2.23 (2.06–2.42) ***2.60 (2.23–3.03) ***2.04 (1.89–2.20) ***2.27 (2.04–2.53) ***
Bad4.54 (4.12–5.01) ***4.56 (3.99–5.21) ***6.75 (4.88–9.33) ***3.60 (2.95–4.40) ***5.19 (3.78–7.11) ***
Monthly income< $1501Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
$1501–$29000.83 (0.79–0.88) ***0.80 (0.73–0.88) ***0.76 (0.61–0.93) **0.85 (0.78–0.93) ***0.87 (0.75–1.01)
$2901–$38000.81 (0.75–0.88) ***0.71 (0.60–0.84) ***0.79 (0.61–1.02)0.86 (0.75–0.98) *0.84 (0.70–1.00) *
>$38000.69 (0.63–0.76) ***0.63 (0.51–0.79) ***0.62 (0.46–0.84) **0.76 (0.65–0.89) ***0.69 (0.57–0.84) ***
Working hours per week41–51Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
<411.00 (0.94–1.05)1.10 (1.00–1.21) *1.16 (0.97–1.38)0.92 (0.85–1.01)0.85 (0.76–0.96) **
52–601.18 (1.10–1.26) ***1.16 (1.03–1.32) *1.15 (0.95–1.39)1.23 (1.11–1.36) ***1.15 (0.99–1.33)
>601.30 (1.20–1.42) ***1.40 (1.19–1.64) ***1.43 (1.09–1.88) **1.26 (1.11–1.44) ***1.28 (1.04–1.57) *
Night work per months0 dayRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
1–10 days1.17 (1.06–1.29) **1.22 (1.04–1.44) *0.95 (0.70–1.28)1.28 (1.07–1.53) **1.09 (0.90–1.32)
Over 10 days1.14 (1.02–1.26) *1.10 (0.92–1.32)0.93 (0.59–1.45)1.14 (0.98–1.33)1.49 (1.12–1.98) **
Work creativity and task varietyLowRef.
High0.87 (0.83–0.92) ***
OccupationBlue collarRef.
White collar0.82 (0.78–0.86) ***
R^20.1170.1210.1180.0750.060
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. $ means dollar.
Table 3. Results of multiple logistic regression related to low subjective well-being according to work creativity and task variety and occupation after adjusting for working-time mismatch, variability, shift work, and autonomy. Odds ratio (95% Confidence Interval).
Table 3. Results of multiple logistic regression related to low subjective well-being according to work creativity and task variety and occupation after adjusting for working-time mismatch, variability, shift work, and autonomy. Odds ratio (95% Confidence Interval).
CovariatesBlue Collar WorkerWhite Collar Worker
Work Creativity and Task Variety
LowHighLowHigh
Model 1Working hours per week41–51Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
<411.21 (1.10–1.34) ***1.13 (0.94–1.36)0.96 (0.87–1.05)0.87 (0.77–0.98) *
52–601.08 (0.95–1.22)1.15 (0.95–1.40)1.19 (1.07–1.32) ***1.14 (0.98–1.32)
>601.24 (1.05–1.46) **1.44 (1.09–1.89) **1.19 (1.05–1.36) **1.26 (1.02–1.55) *
Night work per months0 dayRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
1–10 days1.20 (1.01–1.41) *0.94 (0.70–1.27)1.25 (1.04–1.49) *1.07 (0.88–1.30)
Over 10 days1.11 (0.93–1.33)0.92 (0.59–1.44)1.12 (0.96–1.31)1.47 (1.10–1.95) **
Working-time mismatchAdequateRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
Insufficient1.26 (1.13–1.41) ***1.51 (1.16–1.95) **1.36 (1.20–1.54) ***1.41 (1.17–1.71) ***
Excessive1.58 (1.44–1.74) ***1.10 (0.93–1.30)1.28 (1.18–1.40) ***1.12 (0.99–1.26)
Model 2Working hours per week41–51Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
<411.08 (0.99–1.19)1.16 (0.97–1.38)0.93 (0.85–1.01)0.86 (0.77–0.96) **
52–601.18 (1.04–1.34) **1.16 (0.96–1.41)1.24 (1.12–1.37) ***1.15 (0.99–1.34)
>601.44 (1.22–1.69) ***1.47 (1.12–1.93) **1.28 (1.13–1.46) ***1.29 (1.05–1.59) *
Night work per months0 dayRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
1–10 days1.11 (0.94–1.31)0.85 (0.63–1.16)1.17 (0.98–1.40)1.03 (0.84–1.25)
Over 10 days1.08 (0.90–1.29)0.89 (0.57–1.40)1.11 (0.95–1.30)1.43 (1.08–1.91) *
Working-time variabilityNoRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
Yes1.28 (1.17–1.39) ***1.29 (1.10–1.51) **1.30 (1.18–1.43) ***1.17 (1.03–1.32) *
Model 3Working hours per week41–51Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
<411.10 (1.00–1.21) *1.16 (0.97–1.38)0.93 (0.85–1.01)0.86 (0.76–0.96) **
52–601.16 (1.02–1.31) *1.15 (0.95–1.39)1.22 (1.11–1.36) ***1.15 (0.99–1.33)
>601.42 (1.21–1.66) ***1.44 (1.10–1.89) **1.26 (1.11–1.43) ***1.28 (1.04–1.57) *
Night work per months0 dayRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
1–10 days1.32 (1.10–1.58) **1.04 (0.74–1.44)1.38 (1.15–1.65) ***1.11 (0.90–1.36)
Over 10 days1.19 (0.98–1.45)1.03 (0.64–1.67)1.17 (1.00–1.37) *1.50 (1.13–2.00) **
Shift workNoRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
Yes0.85 (0.73–0.99) *0.80 (0.57–1.14)0.83 (0.73–0.94) **0.95 (0.77–1.18)
Model 4Working hours per week41–51Ref.Ref.Ref.Ref.
<411.11 (1.01–1.22) *1.16 (0.98–1.39)0.93 (0.85–1.01)0.86 (0.77–0.96) **
52–601.16 (1.02–1.31) *1.15 (0.95–1.39)1.22 (1.10–1.36) ***1.13 (0.98–1.31)
>601.38 (1.17–1.61) ***1.42 (1.08–1.86) *1.25 (1.10–1.42) ***1.23 (1.00–1.52) *
Night work per months0 dayRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
1–10 days1.24 (1.05–1.46) *0.95 (0.70–1.28)1.28 (1.07–1.52) **1.08 (0.89–1.31)
Over 10 days1.14 (0.96–1.37)0.94 (0.60–1.47)1.14 (0.97–1.33)1.47 (1.11–1.95) **
Working-time autonomyNoRef.Ref.Ref.Ref.
Yes1.27 (1.16–1.40) ***1.13 (0.94–1.36)1.06 (0.96–1.16)1.21 (1.06–1.37) **
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Odds ratio adjusted for all other confounders (sex, age, education level, numbers of employee, subjective health condition, monthly income).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shin, M.-G.; Kim, Y.-J.; Kim, T.-K.; Kang, D. Effects of Long Working Hours and Night Work on Subjective Well-Being Depending on Work Creativity and Task Variety, and Occupation: The Role of Working-Time Mismatch, Variability, Shift Work, and Autonomy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 6371. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126371

AMA Style

Shin M-G, Kim Y-J, Kim T-K, Kang D. Effects of Long Working Hours and Night Work on Subjective Well-Being Depending on Work Creativity and Task Variety, and Occupation: The Role of Working-Time Mismatch, Variability, Shift Work, and Autonomy. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(12):6371. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126371

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shin, Min-Gwan, Yoon-Ji Kim, Tae-Kyoung Kim, and Dongmug Kang. 2021. "Effects of Long Working Hours and Night Work on Subjective Well-Being Depending on Work Creativity and Task Variety, and Occupation: The Role of Working-Time Mismatch, Variability, Shift Work, and Autonomy" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 12: 6371. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18126371

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop