Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 14 July 2020
Sec. Psychopathology

The Contribution of Rat Studies to Current Knowledge of Major Depressive Disorder: Results From Citation Analysis

  • 1Centro de Filosofia das Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa (CFCUL), Faculdade de Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
  • 2Centre for Environmental and Marine Studies, Departamento de Biologia, Universidade de Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal
  • 3Centre for Research into Ecological and Environmental Modelling, Departamento de Biologia Animal, Centro de Estatística e Aplicações, Faculdade de Ciências, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
  • 4Centre for Animal Welfare, University of Winchester, Winchester, United Kingdom

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is the most severe depression type and one of the leading causes of morbidity worldwide. Animal models are widely used to understand MDD etiology, pathogenesis, and treatment, but the efficacy of this research for patients has barely been systematically evaluated. Such evaluation is important given the resource consumption and ethical concerns incurred by animal use. We used the citation tracking facilities within Web of Science and Scopus to locate citations of original research papers on rats related to MDD published prior to 2013—to allow adequate time for citations—identified in PubMed and Scopus by relevant search terms. Resulting citations were thematically coded in eight categories, and descriptive statistics were calculated. 178 publications describing relevant rat studies were identified. They were cited 8,712 times. More than half (4,633) of their citations were by other animal studies. 794 (less than 10%) were by human medical papers. Citation analysis indicates that rat model research has contributed very little to the contemporary clinical understanding of MDD. This suggests a misuse of limited funding hence supporting a change in allocation of research and development funds targeting this disorder to maximise benefits for patients.

Introduction

Depression is the leading cause of disability worldwide (World Health Organization, 2019). Nowadays, it is judged to affect more than 320 million people of all ages and genders (Vos et al., 2016), even though it is more frequent in women than man (Ferrari et al., 2013).

Currently, and according to DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), there are eight main forms of depression: major depressive disorder (MDD), persistent depressive disorder, premenstrual dysphoric disorder, disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, substance/medication-induced depressive disorder, depressive disorder due to another medical condition, non specified depressive disorder, and other specified depressive disorder. MDD is the most severe, prevalent, and disabling depression type (Malhi and Mann, 2018). It is characterized by a persistent depressed mood or loss of pleasure, along with four out of the following symptoms: significant weight loss or gain; insomnia or hypersomnia; psychomotor agitation or retardation; fatigue or loss of energy; feelings of worthlessness or excessive or inappropriate guilt; diminished ability to think, concentrate, or make decisions; recurrent thoughts of death (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). For MDD to be diagnosed, the patient needs to fulfill five diagnostic criteria out of a pool of nine, which means that the same disorder may present differently in different subjects (Kaufman, 2018). The variety of both symptoms and biomarkers has led to the recent suggestion that there might be several subtypes of MDD (Beijers et al., 2019).

MDD etiology is not completely understood yet. Most authors agree that there is a combination of biological and environmental factors that determine the triggering of the disorder (Mandelli and Serretti, 2013). Biological factors to take into account include genes, neurotransmitters, and hormones, while environmental factors include childhood trauma, stressful life events, sexual abuse, low educational attainment, and differences in personality traits.

Evidence suggests that there are genetic factors involved, but even though more than 100 candidate genes have been investigated, a clear connection between specific genes and MDD has not yet been established (Shadrina et al., 2018). Furthermore, studies suggest that variations in different genes, each with a minor effect, combine to increase the risk of developing this disorder (Wray et al., 2018). Most studies suggest that MDD patients have imbalanced brain chemistry at neurotransmitters level (Beijers et al., 2019). Among those, the majority of the studies indicate dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin as the most implicated in MDD etiology (Belujon and Grace, 2017). Others stress the involvement of glutamate (Réus et al., 2018). Nonetheless, some studies find no differences in neurotransmitters between MDD patients and healthy controls (for a review, see Beijers et al., 2019). Similarly, it is widely accepted that hormones play a role in MDD etiology (Druzhkova et al., 2019) but there is no clear-cut connection between a specific hormone secretion and MDD. For example, Asadikaram et al. (2019) found differences between MDD patients and healthy controls in hormone levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone, testosterone, thyroid–stimulating hormone, free thyroxine index, and cortisol/dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA–S), while others consider vasopressin and oxytocin to play a pivotal role in MDD etiology (for a review, see Iovino et al., 2018).

Recent studies also suggest that there is a link between inflammation and MDD, suggesting that MDD has an inflammatory subtype (Beijers et al., 2019), but the claims that inflammation has a role in etiology of MDD are still being disputed (Miller, 2018). The same happens with changes in gut microbiome in MDD patients (Winter et al., 2018). While the link between gut microbiome and depression is well documented, the question of the causality in the connection between the two remains to be robustly answered (Winter et al., 2018). It is important to mention that these patterns may be true for all biological changes found in MDD patients. It is almost impossible to determine if the biological changes caused MDD or if MDD caused the biological changes. Conversely, most environmental factors involved in MDD are definitely a primary cause. In this regard, the big unanswered question that remains is why does the same life event trigger MDD in one person and not in another.

Among the most documented environmental factors linked to MDD are childhood traumas, which also cause biological changes in the brain of MDD patients (Yu et al., 2019), stressful life events, sexual abuse, low educational attainment (Peyrot et al., 2013), and personality traits (Bensaeed et al., 2014). Other disorders and traits are also strong predictors for MDD. For example, a big longitudinal study showed that people who present anxiety traits in their twenties are more prone to develop MDD in their thirties (Gustavson et al., 2018). Also Parkinson’s, Migrains’, Alzheimer’s patients, among others, have high prevalence of MDD (Ketharanathan et al., 2014; Muneer et al., 2018; Tao et al., 2019).

It is not always possible to determine a proximal cause. MDD may have seasonal or peri-partum onset, as well as being induced by other disorders (e.g., Parkinson’s) or substance ingestion, but it can also emerge without an obvious reason (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Major depressive disorder pathogenesis is as diverse as its etiology. Even though there are several different treatment courses available (for a review, see for example Pandarakalam, 2018), 50–60% of patients develop treatment resistant depression, i.e., do not enter remission even after trying different courses of treatment (Kraus et al., 2019), and only 52% of patients achieve a full recovery (Novick et al., 2017).

Due to its complexity, MDD is particularly hard to study, but its severity, prevalence, and significant economic burden make it a moral and social imperative to keep investing this research field. Yet, its research funding has been scarce when compared to other disorders (e.g., cancer) (Ledford, 2014).

Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are considered to be the gold standard for empirical research (Hariton and Locascio, 2018), namely, in MDD’s potential treatments and interventions (Monsour et al., 2019) but they are unsuitable for all purposes. Some authors consider observational longitudinal studies to be more useful in understanding the etiology and pathogenesis of human disorders (Frieden, 2017) pointing out that they also overcome ethical and practical limitations of RCTs such as the insufficient study duration or the disregard of unpredictable variables that affect patients in their daily lives (Song and Chung, 2010). Others stress the importance of basic and applied research aiming to understand MDD’s mechanisms in a controlled environment (e.g., Papassotiropoulos and De Quervain, 2015).

In this regard, advanced magnetic resonance imaging techniques used in patients and healthy controls can be a powerful tool regarding the physiological and metabolic characterization of brain tissue, in the same way that single photon emission computed tomography and positron emission tomography imaging modalities provide valuable data on brain function and activity (Tsougos et al., 2019).

Post-mortem studies as well as cell-based disease modeling are another valuable set of tools in understanding the biology of psychiatric disorders. As cellular biology techniques evolve new ways to generate and preserve human cells in vitro emerge (e.g., induced pluripotent stem cells, trans differentiation technologies for deriving neurons from adult humans, Vadodaria et al., 2018). However, they are insufficient to fully understand the pathways and progression of complex disorders. Some authors assert that systems biology might be the answer as it can integrate and model different levels of human experimental data—molecular, cellular, tissue, organ, clinical, and population disorders (Langley, 2014). Others suggest a mind-brain paradigm that combines simultaneous use of imaging techniques with the use of scales and/or other tests validated for psychodiagnostics as a way to build a bridge between neuroscience and psychological sciences to study mental disorders (Stoyanov, 2009). Others claim that the only way to overcome such limitations is by resorting to animal models, which are seen by some as crucial for MDD research (e.g., Wang et al., 2017; Akil et al., 2018), despite their well-recognized limitations with respect to human predictability (Akil et al., 2018).

To overcome these limitations, combinations of different animal models are proposed (Akil et al., 2018), different transgenic lines of rats are generated (as described by Bailey, 2019) and efforts are made to overcome the biological differences between species that keep emerging as extrapolation barriers (Hodge et al., 2019). All the above involve high economic costs and consume a tremendous amount of animal lives. The reason behind this is because it is assumed that animal use is unavoidable and its withdrawal would jeopardize human health. However, very few studies have addressed the contribution of animal models to MDD research through significant critical scrutiny within peer-reviewed literature. Specifically, to our knowledge, the contribution of rats for this aim has never been evaluated in such terms, even though rodents are undoubtedly the most frequently used animals regarding this context. Even though mice are by far the most used rodents in biomedical research, an initial search in PubMed, a search engine that comprises more than 30 million biomedical literature publications, indicated that species within genus Rattus were highly used in MDD research, which made them an interesting case study. To evaluate the contribution of animal models to MDD research is important for ethical and economic reasons. As a society, we should make an informed decision on whether we should proceed improving animal models until we find a suitable one or if we should halt the current paradigm and invest more in other methods that might be more promising as well as cheaper and less ethically contentious (Carvalho et al., 2019).

To conduct such evaluation, we performed a citation analysis on original publications describing rat data within MDD research. A citation analysis as defined by Garfield and Merton (1979) consists of determining the number of citations target papers (in this case original papers using rat models to study MDD) receive, as well as determining citation patterns—in this case which sort of papers are citing the target papers (e.g., animal research papers, human research papers, review papers). Granting that the studies cited guide and influence authors’ work (Burright et al., 2005) and that citation level has been related to clinical relevance in the past (Pound and Nicol, 2018), such citation analysis can be used as an indicator to evaluate the contribution of rat studies to current clinical knowledge in MDD, as has been done for other disorders (e.g., Knight, 2007; Long et al., 2014; Carvalho et al., 2016) as well as for other species in regard to MDD research (Carvalho et al., 2019).

If rat studies are informing the human medical research community, then we would expect that:

1. Most of the papers would be cited at least once in subsequent human medical papers;

2. The proportion of citations by human medical papers would be substantially higher when compared to other research categories.

Methods

The citation analysis was performed between January and August of 2019. PubMed and SCOPUS were searched for publications using rat models to investigate MDD. We searched PubMed using Medical Subject Heading search terms (MeSH terms): “Depressive Disorder, Major” AND “rat” OR “rodent.” MeSH terms are a comprehensive list of key terms made available by PubMed designed to identify all relevant studies in an area (Uman, 2011). So, searching for “MDD” retrieves other nomenclatures for the same disorder such as melancholia. Similarly, the search term “rat” retrieves papers using all rat species. We used PubMed filters to exclude review articles (“review,” “systematic review,” “meta-analysis,” “bibliography”) as well as opinion articles (“biography,” “auto-biography,” “comment,” “editorial,” “interview”). Since Scopus does not have the MeSH term tool, we used the search terms “MDD” AND (“rat” OR “rattus”) in the search fields. We included journal papers, books, research reports, and conference proceedings written in English or Portuguese, which are within our language proficiencies. We restricted our search to publications prior to 31 December 2013, to allow adequate time for citation of articles to occur. We did not include a lower date limit.

Since our goal was to evaluate the contribution of animal models—particularly rat models—to current clinical knowledge of MDD, we manually excluded from our analysis all the papers that reported animal and human data, as well as papers reporting other species’ data (e.g., mice). Hence, we included only original papers focused on MDD, exclusively using data obtained from rats. We excluded papers describing other disorders, data from other species along with data from rats, and papers that did not present first hand data (e.g., review papers). Out of 237 papers originally located through the search engines’ filters, we manually excluded 59 because they met exclusion criteria.

The retrieved papers were subjected to a subsequent citation analysis using the cited reference search facility within Scopus and Web of Science.

Web of Science is a major scientific citation indexing service that encompasses over 50,000 scholarly books, 12,000 journals, and 160,000 conference proceedings. Scopus is the largest citation database; it covers nearly 36,377 titles from approximately 11,678 publishers.

For each rat study, we recorded the total number of times it was cited, and allocated each citation to one or more of seven categories, defined prospectively:

– Animals. This category included all animal studies from observational ethological studies to invasive procedures as defined by Knight (2011), i.e., interfering with bodily integrity (whether through puncture or incision) or production of genetically modified animals. This category also included severe procedures (as defined by current European Legislation Directive 2010/63/EU) commonly used in mental disorder research such as inescapable electroshock or isolating social animals for long periods. We recorded within this category which animal papers focused on MDD and which focused on other subjects.

– Humans. This category included papers that used human participants. They included clinical or treatment trials (either drug trials or non-pharmacological treatments), papers aiming to explore psychological, social, biochemical, physiological, genetic, or neurological variables related to MDD or other human disorders; as well as papers aiming to understand the relationship between MDD and other disorders (co-morbidities) in human patients. We recorded within this category which human papers focused on MDD and which focused on other disorders.

– Reviews. This category included narrative reviews, systematic reviews, meta-analysis, as well as extensive opinion papers that did not report original empirical data. We included reviews focused on animals and on humans alike.

– Editorials. This category included editorials, comments, and clinical guides.

In vitro. This category included exclusively cell-line data. Whenever the source of the tissue or cell was a human participant (either alive or post-mortem) or a laboratory animal killed for such purpose the paper was allocated into “human paper” or “animal research paper,” respectively.

In silico. This category included data obtained via computer simulations of human data.

– Social. This category included human surveys or other social perception papers.

Whenever it was not possible to define the category of the citing paper (due to language barriers—i.e., papers written in a language that was not English or Portuguese—or absence of the abstract), the paper was denoted “not available” and removed from the sample. If more than one category could be assigned to a citing paper (e.g., animal research and human paper), then that paper was allocated to every appropriate category.

To evaluate if the proportions of citations made by human publications and animal publications on MDD were different, we used a t-test. Results were considered statistically significant when P < 0.05. The analyses were performed in R 3.6.1 (R Core Team, 2019).

Results

The 178 original rat studies focused on MDD that were published before the end of 2013 were cited 8,712 times by August 2019. Of these 178, 87 (49%) studies were never cited in subsequent publications describing human studies on MDD, and 53 (30%) were never cited in any publications related to human research, either focused on MDD, or on other disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder or bipolar disorder.

As shown in Figure 1, rat studies were mainly cited by other animal research papers (4,641), followed by review papers (2,909), human studies (794), in vitro papers (211), editorials (58), in silico papers (57), and human social papers (one). 230 citations were unavailable to us due to access or language barriers. These were removed from further analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Frequency of citations by category established in this study.

The proportion of citations by human medical papers was 9.1% while the proportion of citations by animal papers was 53.3%. This corresponds to a mean difference between the proportions of citations by human and by animal papers of −46% (p < 0.001). Beyond the statistical significance, this is certainly a considerable practical difference that reflects almost 100(53.3–9.1)/53.3 less citations by human papers than by animal papers.

Discussion

The majority of the rat papers located in this study were cited by subsequent animal research papers, but about half (49%) of the original papers retrieved were never cited in subsequent papers related to MDD in humans, and in fact about a third (30%) were never cited in any subsequent human studies.

Our citation analysis revealed that only around a tenth (9.1%) of the total number of citations were by human medical papers. This result contradicted the hypothesis that the proportion of citations by human medical papers would be substantially higher when compared to other research categories. This hypothesis was based on the assumption that animal models are essential to clinical research, as promulgated by several authors (e.g., Wang et al., 2017; Akil et al., 2018). Hence, our results raise doubts about the justification for these animal studies.

The results of our study are in agreement with previous studies that empirically evaluated the contribution of animal models to human healthcare either through citation analysis (e.g., Shapiro, 1998; Knight, 2007; Long et al., 2014; Carvalho et al., 2016) either through other methods such as systematic reviews (e.g., Perel et al., 2007), social science studies (e.g., Shapiro, 1998; Compton et al., 2019), historical analysis (e.g., Menache, 2012), among others (for an extensive review on this topic see, for example, Knight, 2019).

This suggests that biomedical research resorting to animal models is not normally considered significant, or particularly visible to, the human medical research community.

Supporters of animal models of human disorders claim that this happens: (a) due to differences in the way basic animal work and human clinical trials are conducted, and propose a change to a translational biomarker-based approach within early steps of pre-clinical research (Garner, 2014); or that this occurs (b) due to failings in study design, conduct, analysis, and reporting (as described by Pound and Ritskes-Hoitinga, 2018), which could be resolved with better reporting and better methodological quality (as proposed by Fabian-Jessing et al., 2018).

Opponents of the use of animal models point out that animal models lack external validity, i.e., findings derived in one setting, population, or species cannot be reliably applied to other settings, populations, and species, which is unavoidable since animal models: (a) oversimplify complex human disorders and the conditions in which they occur (Stoyanov, 2009; Pound and Ritskes-Hoitinga, 2018); (b) are unsuitable models due to species differences, proposing as a possible solution a shift toward human-based non-clinical research (Pound and Ritskes-Hoitinga, 2018).

This paradigm change toward human-based research is gaining more and more supporters both for ethical and scientific reasons (e.g., LaFollette and Shanks, 1995; Langley, 2014; Pound and Ritskes-Hoitinga, 2018; Ram, 2019). But the resistance from animal researchers in face of this shift remains significant and is evident not only in the slowness to recognize the growing body of evidence against the use of animals as models, but also in the emphasis placed on refinement of animal use (Franco and Olsson, 2014) (the third R as defined by Russell and Burch, 1959, instead of on the first and most important R—replacement with non-animal alternatives). Considering this, Frank (2005) proposed that animal experimentation constitutes a good example of path dependency, which is a well-documented phenomenon that states that what has occurred in the past persists because of resistance to change.

Our results also show that more than half (53%) of the citations our target papers received were by subsequent animal papers, which strengthens the likelihood of the path dependency phenomenon described above. It can be argued that there is a need to have a substantial amount of animal research before achieving a critical mass that can lead to useful breakthroughs in human health, which might explain the high level of citations of animal papers by subsequent animal papers. Nonetheless, it does not explain the low level of citations by human papers, especially when this does not appear to be a trend in human-based approaches (in vitro and in silico) which received more citations by human medical papers in a small study previously conducted (Carvalho et al., 2019). Furthermore, a recent citation analysis in another field of human health research also found in vitro papers to be the most cited papers, above reviews, and animal experimentations papers (Adnan and Ullah, 2018).

We acknowledge that this study has certain limitations. Even though we used two very large bibliographic databases to attempt to locate all publications that met our search criteria, we acknowledge that a small minority of relevant papers may not have been retrieved (e.g., due to labeling errors) or may have been excluded due to human error, as only one person selected which retrieved papers met exclusion criteria.

Similarly, we did not take note of certain citations such as self-citations, in-house citations (i.e., citations by colleagues from the same insitution), and content-irrelevant citations (e.g., the paper is cited for other reasons other than its content). Such citation types were not considered significant in several studies and were similarly dismissed in recent studies (Huang et al., 2019).

Also, we did not analyze the quality of citing papers as in previous studies (Carvalho et al., 2016, 2019). This might have resulted in an even lower, but a truer, indication of the contribution of rat models for current clinical knowledge of MDD, given that not all were likely of high quality.

Finally, we acknowledge that being cited by human medical papers is not the only indicator of the clinical usefulness of research. Uncited studies may also contribute, for example, through direct transfer of knowledge between scientists and clinicians. Most clinicians are not involved in research, i.e., they do not publish; hence, it is possible that practitioners involved in the treatment of MMD are informed by or utilize findings from rat studies through direct transfer of knowledge. A citation analysis does not measure this effect. However, it is also possible that clinicians are less aware of research results, than researchers. Thus, a survey on practitioners—which we did not perform—would have been an interesting complement to our results.

Review papers on depression that might inspire authors likely cite animal papers, nonetheless if the animal paper is really relevant for the authors’ hypothesis or method it is likely the primary paper will be cited and not solely the review paper that referred to it.

Citation rates may also be affected by factors such as article length, number of authors, their country, and university of affiliation (Leimu and Koricheva, 2005). Despite their limitations, however, citation frequencies do normally provide reasonably objective approximation of the importance of research results within a field. Research that makes a significant contribution—such as by confirming or refuting important hypotheses—is likely to be cited by subsequent papers. Research that is inconclusive or lacking in significance is much less likely to be cited.

Accordingly, we believe that our citation analysis provides a valid and critical assessment of the contribution of rat models to current clinical knowledge of MDD. Such evaluation of the efficacy of research approaches is important to ensure the most responsible allocation of research funds and scientific resources, to maximize the likelihood of clinical benefit to patients, and to minimize the consumption of animal lives in research where such benefits are unlikely.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors on request.

Author Contributions

CC developed the study concept with the supervision of AK, TM, and LV. CC and FP performed data collection. CC and TM made data analysis. All authors contributed to the study design and the draft of the manuscript and approved the final version of the article for submission.

Funding

This study was financed by Animalfree Research—Switzerland, a grant from the Johns Hopkins Center for Alternatives to Animal Testing (CAAT) and by Portuguese National Funds through FCT—Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia, within the CFCUL Unit funding UIDB/00678/2020. TM thanks partial support by CEAUL (funded by FCT—Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia, Portugal, through the project UID/MAT/00006/2019). FP thanks FCT/MCTES for the financial support to CESAM (UIDP/50017/2020 and UIDB/50017/2020), through national funds. Open access publication costs were covered by Animalfree Research-Switzerland and by funding provided by the Ketty and Leif Hjordt Foundation. None of these funders played any role in the study conceptualization, design, conduct, data collection or analysis, authorship of the resulting manuscript or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adnan, S., and Ullah, R. (2018). Top-cited articles in regenerative endodontics: a bibliometric analysis. J. Endod. 44, 1650–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.joen.2018.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akil, H., Gordon, J., Hen, R., Javitch, J., Mayberg, H., McEwen, B., et al. (2018). Treatment resistant depression: a multi-scale, systems biology approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 272–288. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asadikaram, G., Khaleghi, E., Sayadi, A., Foulady, S., Ghasemi, M. S., Abolhassani, M., et al. (2019). Assessment of hormonal alterations in major depressive disorder: a clinical study. PsyCh J. 8, 423–430. doi: 10.1002/pchj.290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, J. (2019). “Genetic modification of animals: scientific and ethical issues,” in Animal Experimentation: Working Towards a Paradigm Change, eds K. Herrmann and K. Jayne (Leiden: Brill), 443-479. doi: 10.1163/9789004391192_020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beijers, L., Wardenaar, K. J., van Loo, H. M., and Schoevers, R. A. (2019). Data-driven biological subtypes of depression: Systematic review of biological approaches to depression subtyping. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 888–900. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0385-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., and Grace, A. A. (2017). Dopamine system dysregulation in major depressive disorders. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 1036–1046. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bensaeed, S., Jolfaei, A. G., Jomehri, F., and Moradi, A. (2014). The relationship between major depressive disorder and personality traits. Iran. J. Psychiatry 9, 37–41.

Google Scholar

Burright, M., Hahn, T., and Antonisse, M. (2005). Understanding information use in a multidisciplinary field: a local citation analysis of neuroscience research. Coll. Res. Lib. 66, 198–211. doi: 10.5860/crl.66.3.198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho, C., Varela, S. A., Bastos, L. F., Orfão, I., Beja, V., Sapage, M., et al. (2019). The relevance of in silico, in vitro and non-human primate based approaches to clinical research on major depressive disorder. Altern. Lab. Anim. 47, 128–139. doi: 10.1177/0261192919885578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho, C., Vieira, C. M., Ferreira, B. L., Knight, A., and Vicente, L. (2016). Contribution of animal models to contemporary understanding of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. ALTEX Altern. Anim. Exp. 33, 243–249. doi: 10.14573/altex.1507311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compton, D. M., Dietrich, K. L., Randall, V. A., and Green, R. E. (2019). Prediction of protocol approval in a mock institutional animal care and use committee: evaluation of type of research protocol, species, and evaluator characteristics. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 7, 1–10. doi: 10.12691/ajap-7-1-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Druzhkova, T., Pochigaeva, K., Yakovlev, A., Kazimirova, E., Grishkina, M., Chepelev, A., et al. (2019). Acute stress response to a cognitive task in patients with major depressive disorder: potential metabolic and proinflammatory biomarkers. Metab. Brain Dis. 34, 621–629. doi: 10.1007/s11011-018-0367-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabian-Jessing, B. K., Vallentin, M. F., Secher, N., Hansen, F. B., Dezfulian, C., Granfeldt, A., et al. (2018). Animal models of cardiac arrest: a systematic review of bias and reporting. Resuscitation 125, 16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.resuscitation.2018.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, A. J., Somerville, A. J., Baxter, A. J., Norman, R., Patten, S. B., Vos, T., et al. (2013). Global variation in the prevalence and incidence of major depressive disorder: a systematic review of the epidemiological literature. Psychol. Med. 43, 471–481. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franco, N. H., and Olsson, I. A. S. (2014). Scientists and the 3Rs: Attitudes to animal use in biomedical research and the effect of mandatory training in laboratory animal science. Lab. Anim. 48, 50–60. doi: 10.1177/0023677213498717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, J. (2005). Technological lock-in, positive institutional feedback, and research on laboratory animals. Struct. Change Econom. Dynam. 16, 557–575. doi: 10.1016/j.strueco.2004.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frieden, T. R. (2017). Evidence for health decision making–Beyond randomized, controlled trials. New Engl. J. Med. 377, 465–475. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1614394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfield, E., and Merton, R. K. (1979). Citation Indexing: Its Theory and Application in Science, Technology, and Humanities, Vol. 8. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Garner, J. P. (2014). The significance of meaning: why do over 90% of behavioral neuroscience results fail to translate to humans, and what can we do to fix it? ILAR J. 55, 438–456. doi: 10.1093/ilar/ilu047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustavson, K., Knudsen, A. K., Nesvåg, R., Knudsen, G. P., Vollset, S. E., and Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2018). Prevalence and stability of mental disorders among young adults: Findings from a longitudinal study. BMC Psychiatry 18:65. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1647-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hariton, E., and Locascio, J. J. (2018). Randomised controlled trials—the gold standard for effectiveness research. BJOG 125:1716. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.15199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodge, R. D., Bakken, T. E., Miller, J. A., Smith, K. A., Barkan, E. R., Graybuck, L. T., et al. (2019). Conserved cell types with divergent features in human versus mouse cortex. Nature 573, 61–68. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1506-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Liu, Y., Liu, H., Hong, X., Guo, X., and Fang, L. (2019). Top 100 most–cited articles on echocardiography: a bibliometric analysis. Echocardiography 36, 1540–1548. doi: 10.1111/echo.14440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iovino, M., Messana, T., De Pergola, G., Iovino, E., Dicuonzo, F., Guastamacchia, E., et al. (2018). The role of neurohypophyseal hormones vasopressin and oxytocin in neuropsychiatric disorders. Endocr. Metab. Immune Dis. Drug Targets 18, 341–347. doi: 10.2174/1871530318666180220104900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, J. (2018). Unraveling the genetics of major depression and stress-related psychiatric disorders: Is it time for a paradigm shift? Biol. Psychiatry 84, 82–84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ketharanathan, T., Hanwella, R., Weerasundera, R., and de Silva, V. A. (2014). Major depressive disorder in parkinson’s disease: a cross-sectional study from Sri Lanka. BMC Psychiatry 14, 278–285. doi: 10.1186/s12888-014-0278-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, A. (2007). The poor contribution of chimpanzee experiments to biomedical progress. J. Appl. Anim. Welfare Sci. 10, 281–308. doi: 10.1080/10888700701555501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, A. (2011). The Costs and Benefits of Animal Experiments. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Knight, A. (2019). “Critically evaluating animal research,” in Animal Experimentation: Working Towards a Paradigm Change, eds K. Herrmann and K. Jayne (Leiden: Brill), 319–340. doi: 10.1163/9789004391192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, C., Kadriu, B., Lanzenberger, R., Zarate, C. A. Jr., and Kasper, S. (2019). Prognosis and improved outcomes in major depression: a review. Transl. Psychiatry 9:127. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0460-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFollette, H., and Shanks, N. (1995). Utilizing Animals. J. Appl. Philos. 12, 13–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5930.1995.tb00116.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langley, G. R. (2014). Considering a new paradigm for Alzheimer’s disease research. Drug Discovery Today 19, 1114–1124. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2014.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ledford, H. (2014). Medical research: If depression were cancer. Nat. News 515, 182–184. doi: 10.1038/515182a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leimu, R., and Koricheva, J. (2005). What determines the citation frequency of ecological papers? Trends in Ecol. Evol. 20, 28–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, X., Huang, J. Z., and Ho, Y. S. (2014). A historical review of classic articles in surgery field. Am. J. Surg. 208, 841–849. doi: 10.1016/j.amjsurg.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malhi, G. S., and Mann, J. J. (2018). Depression. Lancet 392, 2299–2312. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31948-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandelli, L., and Serretti, A. (2013). Gene environment interaction studies in depression and suicidal behavior: an update. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2375–2397. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menache, A. (2012). Are animal models relevant in modern psychiatry. Psychiatric Times 29, 1–5.

Google Scholar

Miller, A. H. (2018). Five things to know about inflammation and depression. Psychiatric Times 35. Available online at: https://www.psychiatrictimes.com/view/five-things-know-about-inflammation-and-depression

Google Scholar

Monsour, A., Mew, E. J., Szatmari, P., Patel, S., Saeed, L., Offringa, M., et al. (2019). Outcomes reported in randomised clinical trials of major depressive disorder treatments in adolescents: a systematic scoping review protocol. BMJ Open 9:e024191. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muneer, A., Farooq, A., Farooq, J. H., Qurashi, M. S., Kiani, I. A., and Farooq, J. S. (2018). Frequency of primary headache syndromes in patients with a major depressive disorder. Cureus 10:e2747. doi: 10.7759/cureus.2747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novick, D., Montgomery, W., Vorstenbosch, E., Moneta, M. V., Dueñas, H., and Haro, J. M. (2017). Recovery in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD): results of a 6-month, multinational, observational study. Patient Prefer. Adher. 11, 1859–1868. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S138750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandarakalam, J. P. (2018). Challenges of treatment-resistant depression. Psychiatria Danubina 30, 273–284. doi: 10.24869/psyd.2018.273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papassotiropoulos, A., and De Quervain, D. J.-F. (2015). Failed drug discovery in psychiatry: time for human genome-guided solutions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 183–187. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perel, P., Roberts, I., Sena, E., Wheble, P., Briscoe, C., Sandercock, P., et al. (2007). Comparison of treatment effects between animal experiments and clinical trials: systematic review. Br. Med. J. 334:197. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39048.407928.be

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peyrot, W. J., Middeldorp, C. M., Jansen, R., Smit, J. H., de Geus, E. J., Hottenga, J.-J., et al. (2013). Strong effects of environmental factors on prevalence and course of major depressive disorder are not moderated by 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms in a large Dutch sample. J. Affect. Disord. 146, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pound, P., and Nicol, C. J. (2018). Retrospective harm benefit analysis of pre-clinical animal research for six treatment interventions. PLoS One 13:e0193758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pound, P., and Ritskes-Hoitinga, M. (2018). Is it possible to overcome issues of external validity in preclinical animal research? Why most animal models are bound to fail. J. Transl. Med. 16, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s12967-018-1678-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Ram, R. (2019). “Extrapolation of animal research data to humans: an analysis of the evidence,” in Animal Experimentation: Working Towards a Paradigm Change, eds K. Herrmann and K. Jayne (Leiden: Brill), 341–375. doi: 10.1163/9789004391192_016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Réus, G. Z., de Moura, A. B., Silva, R. H., Resende, W. R., and Quevedo, J. (2018). Resilience dysregulation in major depressive disorder: Focus on glutamatergic imbalance and microglial activation. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 16, 297–307. doi: 10.2174/1570159X15666170630164715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, W. M. S., and Burch, R. L. (1959). The Principles of Humane Experimental Technique. North Yorkshire: Methuen.

Google Scholar

Shadrina, M., Bondarenko, E. A., and Slominsky, P. A. (2018). Genetics factors in major depression disease. Front. Psychiatry 9:334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, K. J. (1998). Animal Models of Human Psychology: Critique of Science, Ethics, and Policy. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

Google Scholar

Song, J. W., and Chung, K. C. (2010). Observational studies: cohort and case-control studies. Plastic Reconstruct. Surgery 126, 2234–2242. doi: 10.1097/prs.0b013e3181f44abc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoyanov, D. (2009). The cross-validation in the dialogue of mental and neuroscience. Dialog. Philos. Mental Neuro Sci. 2, 24–28.

Google Scholar

Tao, P., Yang, S. N., Tung, Y. C., and Yang, M. C. (2019). Development of Alzheimer disease in old major depressive patients based upon their health status: a retrospective study in Taiwan. Medicine 98:e15527. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000015527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsougos, I., Kousi, E., Georgoulias, P., Kapsalaki, E., and Fountas, K. N. (2019). Neuroimaging methods in epilepsy of temporal origin. Curr. Med. Imaging Rev. 15, 39–51. doi: 10.2174/1573405613666170622114920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uman, L. (2011). Systematic reviews and meta-analyses. J. Can. Acad. Child Adoles. Psychiatry 20, 57–59.

Google Scholar

Vadodaria, K. C., Stern, S., Marchetto, M. C., and Gage, F. H. (2018). Serotonin in psychiatry: in vitro disease modeling using patient-derived neurons. Cell Tissue Res. 371, 161–170. doi: 10.1007/s00441-017-2670-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vos, T., Allen, C., Arora, M., Barber, R. M., Bhutta, Z. A., Brown, A., et al. (2016). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 310 diseases and injuries, 1990–2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. Lancet 388, 1545–1602. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31678-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., Timberlake, M. A. II, Prall, K., and Dwivedi, Y. (2017). The recent progress in animal models of depression. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, G., Hart, R. A., Charlesworth, R. P., and Sharpley, C. F. (2018). Gut microbiome and depression: what we know and what we need to know. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 629–643. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2017-0072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2019). Depression. Available online at: https://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/depression (accessed January 25, 2020).

Google Scholar

Wray, N. R., Ripke, S., Mattheisen, M., Trzaskowski, M., Byrne, E. M., Abdellaoui, A., et al. (2018). Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat. Genet. 50, 668–681. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Linn, K. A., Shinohara, R. T., Oathes, D. J., Cook, P. A., Duprat, R., et al. (2019). Childhood trauma history is linked to abnormal brain connectivity in major depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 8582–8590. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1900801116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: major depressive disorder, animal models, animal use alternatives, citation analysis, rat

Citation: Carvalho C, Peste F, Marques TA, Knight A and Vicente LM (2020) The Contribution of Rat Studies to Current Knowledge of Major Depressive Disorder: Results From Citation Analysis. Front. Psychol. 11:1486. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01486

Received: 08 April 2020; Accepted: 03 June 2020;
Published: 14 July 2020.

Edited by:

Roumen Kirov, Institute of Neurobiology (BAS), Bulgaria

Reviewed by:

Drozdstoy Stoyanov Stoyanov, Plovdiv Medical University, Bulgaria
Kenneth Joel Shapiro, Animals and Society Institute, United States
Pandora Pound, Safer Medicines Trust, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Carvalho, Peste, Marques, Knight and Vicente. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Constança Carvalho, constanca.carvalho@sapo.pt

Present address: Tiago A. Marques, The Observatory, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, United Kingdom

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.